Rowan University Rowan University
Rowan Digital Works Rowan Digital Works
Theses and Dissertations
5-23-2014
The consistency of the use of the polygraph exam during the The consistency of the use of the polygraph exam during the
selection process among law enforcement agencies selection process among law enforcement agencies
Jessica Rachel Mark
Follow this and additional works at: https://rdw.rowan.edu/etd
Part of the Criminology and Criminal Justice Commons, and the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Mark, Jessica Rachel, "The consistency of the use of the polygraph exam during the selection process
among law enforcement agencies" (2014).
Theses and Dissertations
. 551.
https://rdw.rowan.edu/etd/551
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Rowan Digital Works. It has been accepted for inclusion
in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Rowan Digital Works. For more information, please
contact graduateresearch@rowan.edu.
THE CONSISTENCY OF THE USE OF THE POLYGRAPH EXAM DURING
THE SELECTION PROCESS AMONG LAW ENFORCEMENT AGENCIES
by
Jessica Rachel Mark
A Thesis
Submitted to the
Department of Psychology
College of Science and Mathematics
In partial fulfillment of the requirement
For the degree of
Maters of Arts in School Psychology
at
Rowan University
May 6, 2014
Thesis Chair: Roberta Dihoff, Ph.D.
© 2014 Jessica Rachel Mark
Dedication
I dedicate this manuscript to my parents, Joyce and Donald Mark
iv
Acknowledgments
I would like to express my appreciation to Dr. Roberta Dihoff, Dr. Eleanor Gaer,
and Dr. Terri Allen for their guidance throughout this research.
v
Abstract
Jessica Rachel Mark
THE CONSISTENCY OF THE USE OF THE POLYGRAPH EXAM DURING THE
SELECTION PROCESS AMONG LAW ENFORCEMENT AGENCIES
2013/14
Roberta Dihoff, Ph.D.
Master of Arts in School Psychology
The current study examined the use of and the consistency of use of the polygraph
exam by law enforcement agencies throughout the United States as part of the screening
process to find suitable applicants for employment as police officers. The polygraph
exam can be a valuable tool for law enforcement agencies to use in screening applicants,
but research has shown that there are questions with regard to the validity and the
reliability of the polygraph exam and that improvements in the consistency of use and
standardization of practices would increase the validity and reliability of the polygraph
exam as a screening tool (Kircher & Raskin, 1988; Honts & Amato, 2007). A survey
addressing police applicant screening procedures was mailed to the ten largest law
enforcement agencies in the United States. Results indicated that the polygraph exam
was not standardized or used consistently by the law enforcement agencies throughout
the United States in the screening of law enforcement applicants. Seventy percent of the
agencies surveyed used the polygraph exam, and 20% of the agencies administered one
polygraph exam to each applicant. The type of polygraph instrument used for the
polygraph exam was inconsistent among the agencies, and 75% of the agencies disclosed
the results of the polygraph exam to the applicant. The current study provided additional
information to the available research concerning the law enforcement agency applicant
screening process and provided implications and future direction for further investigation.
vi
Table of Contents
Abstract v
List of Figures viii
List of Tables ix
Chapter 1: Introduction 1
Chapter 2: Literature Review 3
2.1 Introduction 3
2.2 History of the Polygraph Exam 3
2.3 Reliability and Validity of the Polygraph Exam 10
2.4 Administration and Evaluation of the Polygraph Exam 12
2.5 Polygraph Exam Use by Law Enforcement Agencies 20
2.6 Standardization of the Polygraph Exam 25
2.7 Implications 28
Chapter 3: Methodology 30
3.1 Subjects 30
3.2 Variables 30
3.3 Procedure 31
Chapter 4: Results 32
Chapter 5: Discussion 39
5.1 Conclusions Regarding the Polygraph Exam Use in Sample Population 39
5.2 Limitations 42
5.3 Future Research Recommendations 43
References 44
vii
Table of Contents (Continued)
Appendix Survey 50
viii
List of Figures
Figure Page
Figure 1 Polygraph Exam Use in Selection Process 32
Figure 2 Polygraph Exam 33
Figure 3 Polygraphs Administered 34
Figure 4 Polygraph Results 34
Figure 5 Procedures Used 35
Figure 6 Appeals 37
Figure 7 Minimum Age of Applicants 38
Figure 8 Minimum College Credits Required 38
ix
List of Tables
Table Page
Table 1 Procedure Order 36
1
Chapter 1
Introduction
The current study focused on the use of the polygraph exam by law enforcement
agencies as part of a screening process to select police officers for employment. The
polygraph exam has been used since the 1920’s to detect lying by measuring
physiological changes in a subject who is being questioned (Green & Heilbrum, 2011).
There is a lack of standardization in test administration, analysis, and evaluation among
law enforcement agencies (Handler, Honts, Krapohl, Nelson, and Griffin, 2009). The
polygraph exam can be a valuable tool for law enforcement agencies to use in screening
applicants, but research has shown that there are questions with regard to the validity and
reliability of the polygraph exam and that improvements in the consistency of use and
standardization of practices would increase the validity and reliability of the polygraph
exam as a screening tool. This study is relevant because police officers are important to
the safety and welfare of society. Fair, accurate, and uniform screening procedures
including the use of the polygraph exam are necessary to ensure that the applicants
chosen to be police officers are good candidates for the job.
The purpose of this study was to examine the use of the polygraph exam by law
enforcement agencies as part of a screening process to find suitable applicants for
employment as police officers. This study focused on whether or not the polygraph test
is standardized and used consistently by law enforcement agencies. A survey addressing
police applicant screening procedures was sent to various law enforcement agencies in
the United States. It was hypothesized that polygraph exams are not standardized or used
2
consistently by selected law enforcement agencies. The study was conducted with regard
to the following operational definition:
Polygraph Exam: an instrument used to record changes in physiological
responses of blood pressure, respiration and dermal response during questioning.
These physiological responses controlled by the autonomic nervous system are
evaluated and provide information about whether the subject is providing truthful
information (Slavkovic, 2002).
The study was also conducted with regard to the following assumptions. Is is
assumed that the questions on the survey were answered truthfully by the selected law
enforcement agencies and that these agencies were a representative sample of law
enforcement agencies in the United States. Limitations of this study included the small
sample size, the low response rate of selected law enforcement agencies, and the
accuracy of the responses to the survey questions as well as the accuracy of the
information obtained from internet law enforcement blogs and websites.
The literature review focused on the history of the polygraph exam, the
components of the polygraph exam, and the procedures of administering the polygraph
exam and evaluating the polygraph exam results. Next, the literature review focused on
problems related to the reliability and the validity of the polygraph exam with particular
emphasis on the use of the polygraph exam during the police applicant screening process.
Reviewed literature also focused on solutions to the problems with the use of the
polygraph exam as a screening tool and how to improve the validity and reliability of the
polygraph exam through consistency and standardization of use.
3
Chapter 2
Literature Review
Introduction
The literature review presents the history of the controversial polygraph exam,
including the legal policies that govern the use of the polygraph exam. Next, the
literature review focuses on problems related to reliability and validly of the polygraph
exam. Then, the literature review focuses on the procedure of administering and
evaluating the polygraph exam including the question techniques and the scoring method.
Particular emphasis is placed on the use of the polygraph exam by law enforcement
agencies in pre-employment screening of police officers and how this screening
procedure lacks standardization.
History of the Polygraph Exam
The literal meaning of the word "polygraph" is many writings (White, 2001). A
polygraph test, more commonly known as a lie detector test, is defined as an instrument
that continuously and simultaneously records changes in cardiovascular, respiratory and
electrodermal activity ("New Rules," 1988). There is no instrument that measures lying.
The changes in the physiological processes that are linked to lying may be occurring in
response to some other psychological factor. The polygraph exam measures
physiological responses or arousal to certain sets of questions (Lewis & Cuppari, 2009).
Polygraphic interrogation is a form of psychological testing based on psychometric
theory and psychophysiology (Lykken, 1974).
The polygraph exam has been used since the 1920's to detect lying by measuring
physiological changes in a subject who was being questioned (Green & Heilbrum, 2011).
4
The search for a reliable way to detect deception has a long history. Since the 19th
century, a wide variety of lie detecting techniques have been developed as scientists have
experimented with instruments to measure deception which they have found to be linked
to physiological arousal. Many studies have been done based on the assumption that
human deception is linked with nervousness, and that liars would exhibit more
nervousness than truth tellers with behaviors such as increased movement or speech
errors (Grubin & Madsen, 2005). Even early societies were interested in finding
procedures and techniques to detect deception in human beings.
Early societies used various forms of torture to detect deception and thought that
deception was linked with emotionality. Some of these early methods to detect deception
were of a physical nature and consisted of activities such as holding a human arm under
boiling water or putting a hot iron on a human hand (Ford, 2006). Evidence of the use of
these methods of detecting deception has been found in ancient Greece, Scandinavia,
Iceland, Japan, and Africa (Grubin & Madson, 2005; Candland & Campbell, 1961). The
idea that deception is linked with nervousness, emotionality and physiological changes in
the human body continued to be investigated in the 19th century.
In the late 19th century, Cesare Lombroso, an Italian criminologist, claimed to be
able to detect deception by monitoring changes in blood volume during questioning of a
subject using a hydrosphygmomanometer, a crude polygraph. In 1914, Vittorio Benussi,
a psychologist, investigated a link between deception and physiological changes such as
blood pressure, pulse, and breathing rate (Gordon, 2008). Hugo Munsterberg also
investigated the link between physiological changes and deception, and a student of
Munsterberg, William Marston invented a deception test based on changes in blood
5
pressure. Marston got the idea for his deception test by observing his wife whose blood
pressure rose when she was angry or excited. Marston reported high positive correlations
between lying and changes in systolic blood pressure using his deception test and used
his test in criminal proceedings (Grubin & Madsen, 2005).
Marston's deception test was used in a criminal proceeding in which a male
named James Frye was accused of robbery and murder. Frye confessed to the crime but
later withdrew his confession claiming that he was bribed to confess. Marston's
deception test found that Frye was truthful about his innocence, but the judge would not
allow Marston's testimony because his deception test was not considered scientific
evidence (United States v. Frye, 1924). Frye was actually later exonerated of his crime
suggesting the validity of Marston's test (Office of Technology Assessment, 1983).
Following the Frye case, the admissibility of deception tests as scientific evidence in
court was not questioned again until 1993.
In 1993, the United States Supreme Court provided guidelines for determining the
admissibility of evidence such as the deception test in court following Daubert v. Merrell
Dow Pharmaecuticals, Inc. (Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, Inc., 1993). The
1993 United States Supreme Court's decision in Daubert v. Merrell Dow
Pharmaceuticals, Inc. modernized the Frye precedent and required courts to make
scientific judgments and apply a broad set of criteria for determining the admissibility of
scientific evidence (Saxe & Ben-Shakher, 1999). In the Daubert case, the Supreme Court
outlined the test for the admissibility of scientific evidence in federal courts (Faigman,
Fiebert, & Stern, 2003). Many federal courts still exclude polygraph evidence because of
6
high error rates and the lack of standards for administering polygraphs (US v. Cordoba,
1999).
In 1998, the United States Supreme Court ruled against the use of the polygraph
as evidence in the case of United States v. Schaeffer. An Air Force man accused of using
illegal drugs wanted to use a polygraph test to prove his innocence, but the Supreme
Court denied his request questioning the reliability of the polygraph (US v. Schaeffer,
1998). Some states in the United States allow polygraph exam results to be admissible
in state courts under certain conditions, and some states do not allow the results to be
admissible in state courts under any conditions. Federal courts follow a different set of
rules regarding the admission of the polygraph exam results into court, and the decision is
left to the judge's discretion (LaMance, 2013).
In 1921, John Larson, a police officer with a Ph.D. in physiology, began
investigating the link between blood pressure and respiratory changes and deception. He
is recognized as having created the first modern polygraph instrument, and his device
made paper recordings of blood pressure, pulse rate, and respiration of a subject who was
being questioned (Kanable, 2010). Larson used his polygraph instrument for criminology
with a question technique later known as the relevant-irrelevant test. The subjects were
presented with questions that were related to the crime and questions that were not related
to the crime while their physiological changes were recorded. Larson's reasoning was
that a guilty subject would exhibit an increased physiological response to the questions
related to the crime, but an innocent subject would not exhibit an increased physiological
response to the questions related to the crime. Although Larson's polygraph instrument
7
showed promise, he became skeptical about the accuracy of his results especially when
used in court (Grubin & Madson, 2005).
In 1939, following the research by Larson, Leonarde Keeler developed the first
portable polygraph instrument that simultaneously recorded pulse rate, blood volume
change, and breathing, but he added the recording of skin response to the instrument.
Keeler's reasoning was that a deceptive subject would sweat more than an honest subject.
The Federal Bureau of Investigation purchased Keeler's instrument, which became the
prototype for the modern polygraph and the first polygraph machine used by a police
department (Grubin & Madsen, 2005; "Lie Detector," 2013).
In the late 1930's, Keeler and John Reid, an attorney, emphasized that while using
the polygraph as an interrogative instrument, the clinical interpretation of the subject's
behavior and the training of the examiner should be considered. Since the interpretation
and training of the examiner were important in the evaluation of a subject's polygraph
test, the test was very subjective and lacked standardization which led to questions
concerning the validity of the polygraph. A student of Keeler and Reid, Cleve Backster,
developed the first numerical scoring system for evaluating the polygraph test, but
numerical scoring did not remove the possible biases of the examiner interpretation
(Grubin & Madsen, 2005).
By the 1940's, the polygraph was used by law enforcement agencies for criminal
investigations and by various government agencies. By the 1960's, the polygraph was
used throughout the United States, and other countries had started polygraph programs.
Bersh (1969) studied the validity of the polygraph exams given to military personnel
during criminal investigations. Cases were randomly chosen from criminal investigations
8
conducted by three branches of service during the years 1963 to 1966. Decisions of guilt
or innocence following a polygraph exam were compared to decisions made by a panel of
military judges who reviewed the case file which did not include the polygraph exam
results. Results showed that the percentages of agreement between the polygraph
examiner and the panel of judges were 92.4%. These results supported the use of the
polygraph exam in this situation (Bersh, 1969). In the United States, businesses in the
private sector began using the polygraph to screen applicants for employment (Office of
Technology Assessment, 1983; Grubin & Madson, 2005).
In 1965, the Committee on Government Operation conducted the first evaluation
of the polygraph and found no scientific evidence to support the use of the instrument to
detect deception (US Congress, 1965). Regardless of this evaluation by Congress, most
large police agencies in the United States began using the polygraph test to screen
applicants and employees during the 1970's (Grubin & Madsen, 2005). In 1983,
President Reagan issued the National Security Decision Directive 84 which authorized
federal agencies to administer the polygraph exam to their employees after a leak of
information regarding the funding of defense plans was found (US Congress, 1983;
Brooks, 1985). The Committee of Government Operations in the United States House of
Representatives requested the Office of Technology Assessment to conduct an evaluation
of the scientific evidence for polygraph tests, and the Office of Technology Assessment
found that the accuracy of the polygraph was undetermined and found no scientific
evidence to support the use of the polygraph test for screening applicants or employees.
The National Security Decision Directive 84 was rescinded (Office of Technology
Assessment, 1983).
9
In 1988, the Employee Polygraph Protection Act legislation restricted the use of
the polygraph test in the private sector, but exempted federal agencies, pharmaceutical
companies, security firm employers, and public service employers, such as law
enforcement agencies (Kanable, 2010). This legislation represented the first time that the
federal government exercised control over the controversial use of the polygraph exam
for screening employees (Kurtz & Wells, 1989). The Act was administered by the United
States Department of Labor's Employment Standards Administration. By the 1990's,
many state police agencies and local police departments required recruits to pass a
background polygraph exam before being hired, since as public service agencies, they
were exempted from the Employee Polygraph Protection Act (Forrer, Mino, & Ehart,
2008).
Polygraph test use continued, and in 1999, the Department of Energy requested a
scientific review of the use of the polygraph test in personnel security screening (Grubin
& Madsen, 2005). The National Academies of Science found that the polygraph test had
significant limitations for screening applications. They found the test to be unreliable,
unscientific, biased and a measure of physiological responses that are not uniquely
related to deception. The National Academies of Science also found that due to the lack
of supporting evidence of reliability and validity, using the polygraph exam for screening
purposes was unjustified (Warner, 2005). Regardless of the additional findings about the
lack of reliability of the polygraph test, the machine continues to be widely used in the
United States and other countries. Some states restrict the use of the polygraph as a
screening tool for possible employment, but few states mandate a total prohibition of the
polygraph exam. Most states do not restrict use of the polygraph exam for law
10
enforcement officers or prospective officers (Herron, 1986). Although private sector use
of the polygraph is more restricted, public sector use continues. Many large companies
and companies engaged in retailing use polygraph testing (Lykken, 1985). Even though
the results of polygraph testing has been found to be not valid and unreliable, law
enforcement agencies still feel that valuable information is provided by polygraph exams
(Warner, 2005). Many federal and local law enforcement agencies use the polygraph test
for criminal investigations as well as for personnel selection (Kanable, 2010; Honts &
Perry, 1992).
The modern polygraph is very similar to the original machine used in the 1920's,
but the modern instrument uses digital technology and has some improvements in
hardware. The polygraph machine has evolved from an analog to a more efficient digital
instrument (Kanable, 2010). Polygraph machines used to be briefcase sized machines
that measured physiological signals by recording the signals with multiple pens on a roll
of paper, but more recent machines consist of a digitizer and a laptop recorder (Meijer &
Verschuere, 2010). Some computerized polygraph systems are used in the United States
(Slavkovic, 2002). The machine still measures physiological responses linked to
deception, the regulation of polygraph examiners and the standardization of the
administration of the test is still problematic, and the test is still considered unreliable by
many scientists (Grubin & Madsen, 2005).
Reliability and Validity of the Polygraph Exam
Despite the technological advances, current methods of psychophysiological
detection of deception are often criticized due to a lack of scientific inquiry and
methodology (Slavkovic, 2002). There is much concern among experts about the use of
11
the polygraph in detecting deception (Ben-Shakhar, Lieblich, & Bar-Hillel, 1982; Saxe,
Dougherty, & Cross, 1985). Humans have motives and emotions and are not passive
recorders and retrievers of information who can be examined effectively with a polygraph
machine. The polygraph exam lacks a sound scientific basis and does not have general
acceptance as a tool to detect deception (Ben-Shakhar, Bar-Hillel, & Lieblich, 1986).
Studies addressing the validity of the polygraph test have not shown reliability or
proper standardization to warrant the use of the polygraph test, particularly in screening
applicants, such as police officers, for employment. Iacono and Lykken (1997) used
surveys to determine the validity of the polygraph. The surveys, sent to the Society for
Psychophysiological Research and the Fellows of the American Psychological
Association addressed many topics relevant to the scientific status of the polygraph. The
results of this study were based only on respondents' self report surveys. Survey return
rates were high, and respondents indicated that polygraph lie detection was not
theoretically sound. The surveys also indicated that claims of polygraph validity were
not sustained and accuracy was affected by the use of countermeasures, such as physical
and mental countermeasures (Iacono & Lykken, 1997).
Many factors must be considered to increase the validity and reliability of the
polygraph exam. Estimates of the validity of polygraph testing, as a method of detecting
deception, range from 90% to 95% by proponents of the polygraph to as low as 61% by
psychologists (Farber, 2011). Proponents of the polygraph claim that the polygraph
accuracy rate is about 90% excluding the inconclusive cases, and the errors show only a
slight preponderance of false positives over false negatives. Critics of the polygraph
claim that the polygraph accuracy is about 70% with most of the errors presented as false
12
positives (Barland, 1985). The American Polygraph Association emphasizes that a valid
polygraph examination requires the combination of a polygraph instrument that records
cardiovascular, respiratory and electrodermal activity operated by a properly trained
examiner using a accepted testing procedure and scoring system (Kanable, 2010).
A study conducted by Horvath and Reid (1971) investigated the reliability of the
polygraph examiner diagnosis. Previous studies have found that polygraph examiners
can successfully diagnosis truth or deception by interpreting only polygraph data. In this
study, experienced and inexperienced polygraph examiners analyzed polygraph data
independently of one another and with only basic factual information about the subject.
The polygraph data from 25 case investigations including guilty and innocent subjects
were presented to the polygraph examiners. The experienced examiners achieved a
91.4% accuracy rate, and the inexperienced examiners achieved a 79.1% accuracy rate in
diagnosing innocence and guilt (Horvath & Reid, 1971). Studies have shown that the
reliability and validity of the polygraph exam is questionable due to the collection of
physiological data, the use of the control question technique on the exam, the use of
countermeasures, and the training of the polygraph examiner who administers and scores
the exam, and yet these exams are used the government, the private sector, in court and
by law enforcement agencies during criminal investigations and to screen applicants.
Administration and Evaluation of the Polygraph Exam
There are 14 major polygraph testing formats used in the United States with some
formats dating from the original in the 1920's. A single-issue test format focuses on one
relevant aspect of a crime. A multifaceted test format focuses on more than one aspect of
a crime. A multi-issue test format contains relevant questions about completely different
13
issues. For example, a multi-issue pre-employment test could present questions about an
applicant's honesty, criminal activity and drug use (Gordon, 2008). The format of the
polygraph, the examiner administering the polygraph and the training and experience of
the examiner all influence the scoring procedure for the test (Slavkovic, 2002).
In the 7-Position Numerical Analysis Scale, scoring depends on spot analysis, and
each question asked during the polygraph exam has a specific location allowing the
examiner to look for changes in the baseline, amplitude, duration, and frequency of the
recorded signals at each spot and compare these changes to the changes at the nearest
control question. Values on the seven point scale ranging from negative three to three are
assigned to the differential of the two responses. The overall score for each spot is
calculated by adding the assigned values, and the grand total is the sum of all spot totals.
Higher reaction to the relevant questions is indicated by the negative values, and higher
response to the control questions is indicated by the positive values. A score of positive
six or greater indicates non-deception, and a score of negative six or lower indicates
deception. Scores in between indicate an inconclusive result (Slavkovic, 2002). These
inconclusive results may be classified as errors causing an examiner to be unable to
render an accurate diagnosis of deception (Kanable, 2010.)
The Control Questions Test and the Guilty Knowledge Test, also known as the
Concealed Information Test, are the two main types of test questions used on polygraph
exams. The control questions polygraph exam asks the subject control questions and
relevant questions. The guilty knowledge polygraph exam measures a subject's
knowledge of specific details related to a specific incident. For example, a guilty
knowledge polygraph exam during a criminal investigation may include questions about
14
different types of weapons, one of which may have been used in the crime. The rationale
is that a guilty subject will react more strongly to questions involving the crime weapon,
but an innocent subject will not react differently to questions involving any weapons.
Important to both types of polygraph exams is the pre-test interview (Lewis & Cuppari,
2009).
The initial interview with the polygraph examiner referred to as the pre-test is
very important to the results of the polygraph exam. During the pre-test phase of the
polygraph exam, the examiner may demonstrate the ability of the machine to detect lies
by asking the subject to tell a simple lie and witness the machine's reactions to the lie.
Usually after the effectiveness of the polygraph machine is demonstrated, truthful
subjects have reduced anxiety, but deceptive subjects have increased anxiety.
Experienced polygraph examiners want to identify truthful subjects as early as possible in
the examination process (Lewis & Cuppari, 2009).
The control question polygraph exam technique assesses a subject's credibility by
looking for differential reactions between control and relevant questions by the subject.
Relevant questions are direct accusatory questions that address the issue under
investigation. Control questions are ambiguous questions to which the subject is
maneuvered into answering in the negative (Honts, 1996). The rationale of using the
control questioning technique on the polygraph exam is that guilty and innocent subjects
will react differentially to relevant and control questions (Honts, Raskin, & Kircher,
1994). Innocent subjects will answer the relevant questions truthfully but are likely to be
uncertain about the truthfulness of their answers to the control questions. Guilty subjects
will answer the relevant questions deceptively but are likely to be more concerned about
15
these questions. Innocent subjects are expected to show stronger responses to the control
questions than to the relevant questions, and guilty subjects are expected to show stronger
responses to the relevant questions than to the control questions (Kircher & Raskin,
1988). Guilty subjects will produce strong physiological reactions when lying to relevant
questions since the questions deal directly with the issues of the exam, such as "Did you
kill the victim?" Innocent subjects will produce strong physiological reactions to control
questions, such as "Before 1970, did you ever do anything dishonest?" Innocent subjects
will produce stronger physiological reactions to the control questions than to the relevant
questions since they are certain of the truthfulness of their answers to the relevant
questions, but they may be lying or uncertain about the truthfulness of their answers to
the control questions (Honts et al., 1994). Analysis and interpretation of the
physiological data recorded by the polygraph is critical in providing validity and
reliability of the results (Slavkovic, 2002).
Thurber (1981) researched the relationship between psychometric data and
control question polygraph performance. The subjects in the research were 34 males in a
police department internship program. The subjects completed the California
Psychological Inventory and a polygraph exam. The results showed that subjects who
scored highly on the Good Impressions subscale of the California Psychological
Inventory also passed the polygraph exam. The Good Impressions subscale assesses
social desirability. This research indicated a connection between social desirability and
the capacity to control the autonomic processes and pass a polygraph exam. Subjects
who behaved in a socially desirable way may have learned to control their autonomic
processes efficiently. Subjects who passed polygraph exams may have been controlling
16
their physiological responses and their autonomic processes which is important to
consider when administering and evaluating the polygraph (Thurber, 1981).
Honts (1996) did a field study of the control question polygraph test using files of
criminal cases. Data from 41 criminal cases was examined for confirming information.
Numerical scores, decisions from the original examiners and an independent evaluation
were analyzed. The results indicated that the control question polygraph technique was
highly valid when excluding the inconclusive answers. This study supported the validity
of the control question polygraph test in real life settings. (Honts, 1996).
Ginton, Daie, Elaad, and Ben-Shakhar (1982) conducted a study to evaluate the
control question polygraph exam in a real-life situation instead of using a mock crime.
The study used 15 male subjects, some of whom actually cheated on an aptitude test
administered at the beginning of the study. Following the test, a polygraph exam was
administered, and each subject was evaluated by three examiners, one who gave the
polygraph exam, one who observed and heard the pretest interview through a one-way
mirror, and one who evaluated the polygraph data results. After the polygraph exams, the
subjects were debriefed. Evaluations based on physiological information were not
superior to those based on the behavioral information only, and evaluations based on both
physiological charts and behavioral information together were more accurate than
evaluations based on either information alone. A limitation of this study was that
although the study simulated a real-life situation for the subjects, the polygraph
examiners knew in advance that their judgments would have no consequences for the
subjects which is not true in real life. Also, due to the nature of this study in finding real-
life cheaters, there was difficulty obtaining a larger sample size (Ginton et al., 1982).
17
A meta-analysis of 14 mock crime studies of the control question polygraph exam
was conducted by Kircher, Horowitz, and Raskin (1988). They found that accuracy in
these studies depends on whether the subjects, the incentives provided to the subjects,
and the methods used for the evaluation of physiological data accurately represent field
conditions. In a mock crime study, the laboratory has advantages over the field, but the
results may not be representative of the field (Podlesny & Raskin, 1977). Bradley, Malik,
and Cullen (2011) found that laboratory mock tests with concealed information and the
polygraph exam resulted in overestimates of detection since laboratory studies foster high
recall on the part of the subject (Bradley et al., 2011). The real-life consequences of
failing a polygraph test and the motivations of subjects to be truthful are much greater in
the field than in the laboratory. There also may be differences in the qualifications of the
examiners of the polygraph test with regard to those done in the laboratory and those
done in the field which would affect the interpretation and diagnosis. In addition to these
differences, in these mock crime control question polygraph studies, the amount of
physiological data provided to the polygraph interpreter varied across experiments
(Szucko & Kleinmuntz, 1981). This meta-analysis found that conducting mock crime
experiments that closely resemble field conditions is crucial in providing accurate
polygraph exam information. More studies on the control question polygraph exam with
computerized standardization of scoring are necessary (Kircher et al., 1988).
Polygraph exams using control questions, which are specially formulated in a
pretest interview, are the most commonly used polygraph tests in law enforcement, but
simple countermeasures such as physical maneuvers during questioning enhances the
physiological reactions to control questions. A physical maneuver such as tongue biting
18
may help a subject pass a polygraph (Honts et al., 1994). Examinees may try to alter the
results of a polygraph exam by physical or mental processes. Physical countermeasures
consist of the subject engaging in some type of pain to mask the physiological response,
and mental countermeasures consist of the subject altering the thought patterns in order to
alter the results of the polygraph exam (Lewis & Cuppari, 2009).
Honts et al. (1994) did a study investigating the effects of mental and physical
countermeasures on polygraph tests and found that both countermeasures were equally
effective in defeating control question polygraph tests and passing these tests (Ginton et
al., 1982). The Honts study was conducted using 120 male and female subjects who were
randomly assigned to groups. One group was innocent of the mock crime, and one group
enacted the mock crime of stealing a rare coin. Some of the subjects received mental
countermeasure training, and some of the subjects received physical countermeasure
training. The results strongly suggest that control question polygraph tests may be
defeated by guilty subjects trained in mental or physical countermeasures. About one
half of the subjects in each countermeasure training condition passed their polygraph test
with even just thirty minutes of countermeasure training. The polygraph examiner
detected only 12% of the physical countermeasures and none of the mental
countermeasures. This a valuable study since countermeasures are a factor that
contribute to the high rate of false negative errors obtained in polygraph screening (Honts
et al., 1994).
In another study, Honts, Hodes, and Raskin (1985) investigated the effects of the
use of physical countermeasures on physiological detection of deception and found that
training in the use of physical countermeasures affected the outcome of a polygraph
19
exam. The study was conducted using 65 male and female subjects, some of whom
enacted a mock crime and were trained in the use of physical countermeasure to be used
during the administration of a polygraph exam. Countermeasure subjects were able to
defeat the control question polygraph exam by producing physiological responses that
were larger to control questions than to relevant questions, and the physical
countermeasures used to produce these physiological responses were undetected by the
examiner (Honts et al., 1985).
Honts, Devitt, Winbush, and Kircher (1996) did a study to investigate the effects
of mental and physical countermeasures on the concealed knowledge polygraph exam
and found that using these countermeasures reduced the accuracy of the concealed
knowledge polygraph exam. The study used 40 subjects and a mock crime scenario with
some subjects receiving countermeasure training and others not receiving the training.
Not only did the use of countermeasures reduce the accuracy of the polygraph exam, but
the use of countermeasures was undetected by the examiner. The results of this study are
consistent with other studies that show that control question and the concealed knowledge
polygraph exam results are influenced by the use of counter measures (Honts et al.,
1996).
The control question polygraph exam is used frequently particularly among law
enforcement agencies for the purpose of screening applicants to be police officers, but the
validity of this type of interrogation is not certain and remains controversial (Ginton et
al., 1982). The control question polygraph exam is widely used, but is very controversial
since the diagnoses made from the results rely on human interpretation which is
influenced by the use of countermeasures, bias, training and experience of the examiner.
20
In order to minimize the effect of interpretation of polygraph protocols and promote
standardization of practice, Kircher and Raskin (1988) investigated the feasibility of
using computer programs to quantify physiological responses to test questions on a
polygraph exam and to assess the probability that the questions were answered truthfully.
In the investigation, computer analysis was used to evaluate physiological responses and
assess the probability of truthfulness in data obtained from two mock crime experiments
involving subjects who were either guilty or innocent of committing a mock theft. The
computer analysis was compared to human numerical evaluations, and no significant
differences between computer and human evaluations were found (Kircher & Raskin,
1988).
A study by Honts and Amato (2007) investigated the automation of a screening
polygraph test. The study examined the effects of automating the relevant-irrelevant
psychophysiological detection of deception test using a mock screening scenario. Eighty
participants were either truthful or deceptive on an employment application and then
administered a polygraph exam by an experienced polygraph examiner or with a fully
automated polygraph exam. The automated polygraph exam produced significantly more
accurate results than the polygraph exam administered by the human examiner, and the
automated method significantly discriminated deceptive from truthful responses (Honts
& Amato, 2007).
Polygraph Exam Use by Law Enforcement Agencies
Most law enforcement agencies use the polygraph exam regardless of reliability
to gain additional information especially in criminal investigations. A study conducted
by the Federal Bureau of Investigation's polygraph unit showed that out of the 2,641
21
deceptive criminal polygraph reports reviewed, 1,316 provided no additional useful
information, but 1,315 reports resulted in acquiring confessions, admissions, or
information of investigative value (Warner, 2005). A meta-analysis was conducted to
determine individual human differences in the ability to detect deception without the use
of the polygraph. Using 108 studies done between 1970 and 2004, Aamodt and Custer
(2006) found that age, confidence, experience, education and gender of the examiner
were not significantly related to the rate of accuracy in detecting deception. Of interest
was the finding that police officers, detectives, judges and psychologists were no more
accurate at detecting deception than other individuals not involved in these fields
(Aamodt & Custer, 2006).
Law enforcement pre-employment polygraph screening exams are a decision-
support tool which should be used in conjunction with other screening tools to help
increase the validity of the selection process. Problems surrounding the polygraph exam
such as the misunderstanding of the test, ineffective selection of test issues, poorly
constructed test questions and misguided policies addressing the use of the polygraph
decrease the validity and reliability of using the polygraph exam as part of the screening
process (Handler, Honts, Krapohl, Nelson, & Griffin, 2009). Polygraph exams are used
in both the pre-conditional and post-conditional offer stages in law enforcement. The
pre-employment evaluation stage of law enforcement may involve the integration of an
applicant's behavioral history information which will be accessed through a personal
history questionnaire, psychological testing, interviews and a polygraph exam (Ben-
Porath et al., 2011). Before a polygraph exam is administered, an applicant should meet
the requirements set forth by the Board of the American Polygraph Association (2012) to
22
be deemed suitable for the exam ("Model Policy for the Evaluation of Examinee
Suitability for Polygraph Testing," 2012).
In 1995, the American Polygraph Association conducted a survey to determine
the use of pre-employment polygraph testing in 626 law enforcement agencies
throughout the United States and found that 62% of the respondent agencies utilized the
polygraph exam as part of their applicant screening process. Most respondent law
enforcement agencies reported that the polygraph exam was as useful a tool for screening
applicants as background investigation, written psychological tests, psychological
interviews, personal interviews and interviews by a selection board, since they were able
to reject approximately a quarter of their applicants as a result of information obtained
through the polygraph exam that had not been uncovered with the other screening
procedures (Handler et. al., 2009).
In 1995, Sanders, Hughes, and Langworthy (1995) did a survey of police officer
recruitment and selection in major police departments in the United States and found that
between 1990 and 1994 there was an slight increase in the use of the polygraph from 69%
in 1990 to 69.5% in 1994 (Sanders et al.,1995). The Law Enforcement Management and
Administrative Statistics (LEMAS) survey is a nationally representative sample of state
and local law enforcement agencies (Burch, 2012). A 2000 LEMAS survey found that
67% of primary state law enforcement agencies in the United States used the polygraph
exam (U.S. Department of Justice, 2000). In 2003, about 45% of local police
departments in the United States used the polygraph exam in the screening process. In
2007, polygraph exams were used by 50% of local police departments in the United
States (Reaves, 2010). These statistics show an increase in the use of the polygraph exam
23
among law enforcement agencies which indicates that the exam is considered to be a
useful tool available to law enforcement agencies to use in obtaining information
concerning criminal activity or information about police officer applicants.
Screening polygraph questions used by law enforcement agencies test for
credibility about involvement in specified patterns of behavior which take place over long
periods of time. Criminal investigators use the polygraph exam to get information about
specific events, but questions on screening polygraph exams refer to the subject's entire
lifetime or their entire adult lifetime which seems to decrease reliability due to the
reliance on a much longer period of remembering. Pre-employment polygraph exams are
conducted in the absence of a known incident in contrast to criminal investigative
polygraph exams which focus on a subject's involvement in a single known event
("Model Policy for Law Enforcement/Public-service Pre-employment Polygraph
Screening Examinations," 2012). Screening polygraph exams involve investigation of
multiple behavioral or suitability topics instead of a known incident, which means that
the subject could be deceptive to one or more relevant issues while simultaneously being
truthful to others (Podlesny & Truslow, 1993). Questions on a screening polygraph exam
should all meet accepted criteria, and behaviors referred to should be supported by clear
definitions that are understood by the examinee and the examiner (Handler et al, 2009).
The questions on screening polygraph exams should provide predictive validity
concerning job performance and be presented in a structural way that is thorough and
non-confrontational. Studies have shown that relevant structured interviews produce
more accurate predictive results (Cortina, Goldstein, Payne, Davison, & Gilliland, 2000;
McDaniel, Whetzel, Schmidt, & Maurer, 1994). The base rate of the relevant questions
24
on the polygraph screening exams creates problems with interpretation and affects
validity and reliability. For example, a question about felonies may have a low base rate
of occurring, but a question about illegal drug use may have a high base rate of occurring.
These differences affect the interpretation of and the reliability of the test (Handler et al.,
2009). A diagnostic test analyzing base rate change is available, but behaviors with low
or high base rate occurrence are not effectively discriminated using the polygraph test,
and do not aid the interpreter in selecting suitable and unsuitable applicants (Wells &
Lindsay, 1980; Wells & Olson, 2002). The results of the polygraph exam become more
valid if the test questions meet standard, accepted criteria (Handler et al., 2009). In 2007,
the American Polygraph Association adopted a standard of practice effective January 1,
2012 that requires members to use validated physiological detection of deception
examination techniques that meet certain levels of criterion accuracy which means that
the test results have to correspond to what the test is designed to detect (Gougler et al.,
2011).
There are two types of errors, false positives and false negatives, common to
polygraph exams. A positive result signifies that the subject was involved with a specific
behavior, and a negative result signifies that the subject was not involved with a specific
behavior. A false positive results from a truthful subject being judged as deceptive by the
examiner, and a false negative results from a deceptive subject being judged as truthful
by the examiner. Reliable and valid screening polygraph exam questions should be
sensitive enough to the important issues to avoid false positives and false negatives.
Screening polygraph exam questions should also be very specific with regard to the
issues of concern in order to avoid the possibility of questions addressing unrelated issues
25
causing false positives. These types of errors on polygraph exams contribute to the lack
of reliability and validity of the test. In order to maximize the reliability and validity of a
screening polygraph test, questions should be empirically related to hiring decisions and
behaviors that are performance predictors (Handler et al., 2009).
Standardization of the Polygraph Exam
A major contribution to the lack of reliability and validity of the screening
polygraph exam is the lack of standardization in the interpretation of the polygraph exam.
Polygraph examiners should use analysis procedures based on scientific reasoning, but
interpretation is done by inspection of the data for physiological reactions or changes in
reaction trends that occur in response to repeated presentations of a specific question
which is limited to an examiner's impressions. Research shows that the rate of human
detection of deception is only about 54% (Aamodt & Custer, 2006). A screening
polygraph examiner may also use a numerical method aided by a computer-based
statistical analysis for interpreting the polygraph data, but this type of interpretation is
standardized from forensic settings and not accurately generalizable to screening settings.
In a numerical scoring, for a subject to be considered truthful, all test questions must
produce truthful answers, but to be considered as lying, only one untruthful answer is
necessary. Screening polygraph examiners rely on hand-scoring systems and their
opinions to arrive at a score (Handler et al., 2009). Polygraph examiners in the United
States are not well trained, and there is a lack of standardization of the process of
administering and interpreting the polygraph exam (Honts & Perry, 1992)
The validity, accuracy, and reliability of pre-employment polygraph exam
screening do not seem to be sufficient enough to justify the use of the polygraph exam as
26
a screening tool for law enforcement agencies. There is not enough research
investigating the accuracy of polygraph based pre-employment decisions, and test
outcomes are not directly related to employment decisions. More regulation and
standardization of the polygraph exam and screening process is necessary. The examiner
should be licensed, and there should be accountability with regard to the interpretation of
the polygraph results. There should be standard rules and regulations regarding what is
and what is not permissible practice during the process of administering the exam and
what course of action is available if the rules are not followed. Following standard
procedure would increase the validity of the polygraph exam particularly when used as an
applicant screening tool for law enforcement agencies (Horvath, 1985).
Some law enforcement agencies in the United States require police officer
applicants to have up to 60 college credits, and most state and local government agencies
require an applicant to have a high school diploma, be 21 years old, pass a written
examination, pass a physical exam, be a United States citizen, pass a psychological
evaluation and pass a polygraph exam (Woska, 2006). The use of the polygraph exam in
screening job applicants is subject to abuses. There must be standard rules to be followed
to protect the employer and the applicant in the investigative process. This may be
accomplished through regulation and standardization of the entire process of
administering a polygraph exam. Only experienced, licensed examiners should
administer a polygraph exam, and the exam should be standardized with clear
instructions given before, during and after the exam (Hurd, 1985).
The polygraph examiner should have an acceptable background and
qualifications. Education, training, experience and ethical principle standards and
27
requirements should be regulated by laws (Yeschke, 1965). The polygraph exam is a
complex and comprehensive exam, and the results are influenced by many factors such as
technique implemented, population tested, context of the exam, examiner training, and
goals of detection all of which should be regulated and standardized. The results are a
function of both science and the ability of the examiner to consider the many
psychological factors before making a decision about a subject's truthfulness. The role of
the examiner is critical to both the administration and the interpretation of the polygraph
exam (Lewis & Cuppari, 2009). Problems with the polygraph exam are compounded by
the complexity of the physiological response protocols and the lack of standardized
generally accepted methods for evaluation (Kircher & Raskin, 1988).
The Kircher and Raskin (1988) study found that computer techniques may be
developed for applied settings and would perform at least as well as expert human
interpreters. Computer analysis to detect truth and deception in applied settings would
not depend on the expertise and objectivity of polygraph interpreters. Use of an
automated polygraph exam method would reduce the risk of errors of human judgment,
and minimize disagreements among polygraph examiners and experts concerning the
polygraph outcome. Using an automated system for administering and evaluating
polygraph exams would improve the reliability and accuracy of polygraph exam results
particularly in job screening. A computerized evaluation could determine whether a
subject is truthful or deceptive and the probability of truthfulness which is more
informative than a categorical judgment and is readily understood and communicated
(Kircher & Raskin, 1988).
28
Much of the criticism of the use of the polygraph exam is concerned with the
examiner with regard to training and bias. This problem could be resolved with the use
of an automated system which would be more standardized, reliable and valid. A
computer would interactively present the examination and collect the data. Studies by
Kircher and Raskin (1988) and Honts and Amato (2007) have shown positive results
using automated polygraph systems. With the use of automated polygraph exam systems,
issues of examiner bias would be removed and reliability and validity of test results
would be improved (Kircher & Raskin, 1988; Honts & Amato, 2007). More research on
automation systems is needed.
Implications
The polygraph exam has been used in some form for over 90 years. From the
beginning of its use, the polygraph has been utilized as a lie detector to detect
physiological changes in the human body that are thought to be linked with deception.
Research has shown that these physiological changes may be due to other psychological
processes and not simply lying. There are many controversial problems with the
administration of the polygraph exam, such as type of test questions and the use of
countermeasures to pass the exam. Scoring and evaluation of the polygraph exam are
also problems since there does not seem to be a standardization of these processes. There
also does not seem to be standardization with regard to the examiner and the individual
who evaluates the polygraph exam. More consistency and standardization of polygraph
exam procedures as well as automated administration and computerized analysis would
increase the validity and reliability of the polygraph exam as a screening tool. Even
though research has shown that the polygraph exam may not be a reliable, valid tool to
29
detect deception and has not been used consistently, the polygraph exam is used by the
government, by the private sector, and by law enforcement agencies for criminal
investigations as well as for employment screening (Green & Heilbrum, 2011; Grubin &
Madsen, 2005; Slavkovic, 2002; Iacono & Lykken, 1997; Kircher & Raskin, 1988).
The purpose of the current study was to examine the use of the polygraph exam
by law enforcement agencies as part of the screening process to find suitable applicants
for employment as police officers and to determine if the polygraph exam is used
consistently by law enforcement agencies throughout the United States. The screening
process for law enforcement applicants is an important procedure since these applicants
have the potential to become enforcers and protectors of the safety and welfare of society.
It was hypothesized that polygraph exams are not standardized or used consistently by
law enforcement agencies.
30
Chapter 3
Methodology
Subjects
The subjects were the ten largest law enforcement agencies in the United States.
These agencies had the largest number of sworn police department employees. Survey
were mailed to New York City, New York, Chicago, Illinois, Los Angeles, California,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, Houston, Texas, Washington, D. C., Phoenix, Arizona,
Dallas, Texas, Miami-Dade, Florida, and Detroit, Michigan. The survey addressing law
enforcement agency employment screening procedures was completed by an employee of
the law enforcement agency and returned in the stamped addressed envelope which was
enclosed. The surveys were kept anonymous, the participants were not compensated,
there was no risk involved, and the law enforcement agencies represented various
demographic areas of the United States.
Variables
A survey (see Appendix) research technique was used to obtain data. The survey
research technique looked at the consistency of the use of the polygraph exam by law
enforcement agencies in the selection of applicants. The survey, similar to a survey used
by Chang-Bae (2006) in his study on psychological testing for recruit screening, was
created specifically for the current study and addressed polygraph exam use for police
applicant screening. Questions pertaining to the use of the polygraph exam, the
administration of the polygraph exam, the scoring and analysis of the polygraph exam,
and the disclosure of the results of the polygraph exam were included in the survey. The
survey addressing police applicant screening procedures was mailed to the law
31
enforcement agencies in New York City, New York, Chicago, Illinois, Los Angeles,
California, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, Houston, Texas, Washington, D. C., Phoenix,
Arizona, Dallas, Texas, Miami-Dade, Florida, and Detroit, Michigan in order to obtain
data on law enforcement agency employment screening procedures. Included in the
mailing was an addressed, stamped envelope in which the survey was returned to the
researcher.
Procedure
A survey, addressing police applicant screening procedures, was mailed to each of
the ten participant law enforcement agencies. The subjects read an alternate consent form
and completed and returned the survey to the researcher in an included stamped envelope,
and all responses were kept anonymous. Information and data were also obtained
through reading law enforcement blogs, sending emails, reading websites, and telephone
calls to law enforcement agencies. After receiving the returned surveys, the responses
were recorded and analyzed. All of the participants were treated according to ethical
standards. The data obtained from the responses of the law enforcement agencies was
analyzed using descriptive statistics. These statistics reflected the consistency of the use
of the polygraph exam during the police applicant selection process.
32
Chapter 4
Results
The current study examined the use of and the consistency of use of the polygraph
exam by law enforcement agencies throughout the United States as part of the screening
process to find suitable applicants for employment as police officers. The administration
of the polygraph was investigated, and results showed that there was inconsistency with
regard to the type of polygraph used, the automation of the polygraph, the number of
polygraphs administered per applicant, the scoring procedure of the polygraph exam, and
the disclosure of the results to the applicant.
As shown in Figure 1, the polygraph exam was used by 70% (n=7) of the
agencies surveyed, and all of the agencies that responded to the questions regarding the
administration of the polygraph reported that the polygraph exam was administered by a
polygraphist.
Figure 1. Polygraph exam use in selection process.
Used: 70%
Not Used: 30%
33
Twenty percent (n=1) of the agencies reported that a police sergeant analyzed the
polygraph exam results, and 80% (n=4) of the agencies reported that a polygraphist
analyzed the polygraph exam results, but the qualifications of the polygraphist, or
polygraph examiner, and the police sergeant were not stated. The type of polygraph
instrument used was inconsistent among the agencies that responded to the survey (n=4),
with two agencies using the Lx-4000, one agency using the LPDT, and another agency
using different polygraph instruments for different polygraph exams. As shown in Figure
2, of the agencies that responded to the surveys, 75% used an automated polygraph exam
(n=3), and 25% of the agencies did not use an automated polygraph exam (n=1).
Figure 2. Polygraph exam.
An automated polygraph is one in which a technician oversees an automated system that
administers the polygraph exam as a standardized test with tape recordings. As Figure 3
shows, of the agencies that responded to the surveys, 20% of the agencies administer one
Automated: 75%
Not Automated: 25%
34
polygraph exam to each law enforcement applicant (n=1), and 80% of the agencies
administer one or two polygraph exams to each applicant (n=4).
Figure 3. Polygraphs administered.
Of the agencies that use the polygraph and responded to the survey (n=3), all of the
agencies use both human and computer scoring procedures for the polygraph exam. As
shown in Figure 4, of the agencies that responded to the surveys, 75% disclosed the
results of the polygraph exam to the applicants (n=3), and 25% of the agencies did not
disclose the results of the polygraph exam to the applicants (n=1).
Figure 4. Polygraph results.
One Polygraph
Administered to One
Applicant: 20%
Results Disclosed to
Applicant 67%
Results Not
Disclosed to
Applicant 33%
35
The procedures, the order in which the procedures are administered, and the
appeal process utilized by the ten largest law enforcement agencies in the United States
were also investigated. The screening procedures, in addition to the polygraph exam,
used by law enforcement agencies were the background investigation, the criminal record
check, the driving record check, the medical exam, the drug test, the physical agility test,
the credit history check, the written aptitude test, the personal interview, and the
psychological evaluation. As shown in Figure 5, the most frequently used procedures
used by all of the ten agencies surveyed were the background investigation, the medical
exam, and the psychological evaluation.
Figure 5. Procedures used.
The drug test, the written aptitude test, and the personal interview were used by 90% of
the agencies surveyed (n=9). The criminal record check was only used by 70% (n=7) of
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
Number of Agencies
Procedure
36
the agencies surveyed. The driving record check was used by 60% (n=6), and the credit
history check was used by 30% (n=3) of the agencies surveyed.
The order in which the procedures were administered by law enforcement
agencies was investigated. As shown in Table 1, the agencies varied in the order that
they each administered the procedures.
Table 1
Procedure Order
Procedure
1
st
2
nd
3
rd
4
th
5
th
6
th
7
th
8
th
9
th
10
th
11
th
Background Investigation
10%
10%
50%
10%
20%
Criminal Record Check
17%
33%
33%
17%
Driving Record Check
20%
40%
20%
20%
Medical Exam
20%
10%
30%
10%
10%
10%
10%
Drug Test
11%
22%
22%
11%
11%
22%
Physical Agility Test
10%
10%
20%
10%
10%
20%
20%
Credit History Test
100%
Written Aptitude Test
67%
11%
22%
Personal Interview
22%
22%
22%
11%
11%
11%
Polygraph Exam
29%
14%
14%
29%
14%
Psychological
Evaluation/Personality
Inventory
10%
10%
10%
10%
10%
20%
20%
10%
The results indicated that the background investigation was administered by the agencies
as the fifth step of the procedures 50% (n=5) of the time. The written aptitude test was
administered by the agencies as the first step of the procedures 67% (n=6) of the time.
The polygraph exam was administered by the agencies as the third, fourth, sixth, seventh,
or eighth steps of the procedures. The polygraph exam was administered by the agencies
as the third step of the procedures 29% (n=2) of the time and was administered by the
agencies as the seventh step of the procedures 29% (n=2) of the time. The polygraph
exam was administered as the fourth step of the procedures 14% (n=1) of the time, as the
37
sixth step of the procedures 14% (n=1) of the time, and as the eighth step of the
procedures 14% (n=1) of the time.
Whether or not the agencies allowed appeals for any of the procedures of the
selection process was examined. As shown in Figure 6, of the agencies that responded to
the surveys, 86% (n=6) allowed appeals for any of the procedures of the selection
process, and 14% (n=1) of those agencies did not allow appeals for any of the procedures
of the selection process.
Figure 6. Appeals.
Minimum age of the applicant and minimum number of college credits attained by the
applicant were also investigated. As shown in Figure 7, results indicated that 50% (n=5)
of the agencies required a minimum age of 21 for applicants, 30% (n=3) of the agencies
required a minimum age of 19 for applicants, and 20% (n=2) of the agencies required a
minimum age of 18 for applicants.
Appeals Allowed: 86%
Appeals Not Allowed:
14%
38
Figure 7. Minimum age of applicants.
As shown in Figure 8, results indicated that 50% (n=5) of the agencies required a
minimum of 60 college credits for applicants, 10% (n=1) of the agencies required a
minimum of 48 college credits for applicants, and 40% (n=4) of the agencies did not
require any college credits.
Figure 8. Minimum college credits required.
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
Minimum Age
Agency
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
60
48
0
Number of Agencies
Number of College Credits
39
Chapter 5
Discussion
Conclusions Regarding the Polygraph Exam Use in Sample Population
The purpose of the current study was to investigate the use of and the consistency
of use of the polygraph exam as part of the screening process for prospective applicants
for employment as police officers by law enforcement agencies throughout the United
States. It was hypothesized that polygraph exams are not standardized or used
consistently by law enforcement agencies. The results of the current study supported the
hypothesis and indicated that the polygraph exam is not standardized or used consistently
by law enforcement agencies throughout the United States as part of the applicant
screening process.
The results showed that there was inconsistency with regard to the type of
polygraph instrument used, the automation of the polygraph, the number of polygraphs
administered per applicant, the scoring procedure of the polygraph exam, and the
disclosure of the polygraph exam results to the applicant. Not all of the agencies
surveyed used the polygraph exam, but all of the agencies that used the polygraph exam
administered the exam using a polygraphist who may or may not be licensed. Some
agencies reported that a police sergeant analyzed the polygraph exam results while other
agencies reported that a polygraphist analyzed the polygraph exam results, but the
qualifications of the polygraphist and the police sergeant were not stated. The type of
polygraph instrument used was not consistent among the agencies, and some agencies
used automated polygraph exams while other agencies did not use automated exams.
Some agencies administered one polygraph exam to applicants while others administered
40
one or two polygraph exams to each applicant. Scoring of the polygraph exam was done
by human and computer scoring procedures among all of the agencies surveyed, and
reliability issues exist with both types of scoring. Some of the agencies disclosed the
results of the polygraph exam to the applicants while others did not disclose the results of
the polygraph exam.
The agencies were also not consistent in the use of screening procedures other
than the polygraph in the employee selection process. There were differences in which
procedures were used, in the order in which the procedures were administered, in whether
appeals were permitted in the process, in the minimum age requirement, and in the
minimum amount of college credits required among the law enforcement agencies. Since
police officers are valuable to the welfare of society, the law enforcement applicant
screening process should be fair, accurate, consistent, and should ensure that the best
candidates are chosen for the job.
The results of the current study agree with previous research since inconsistencies
were found with regard to the use of the polygraph exam as well as with other screening
tools in the screening process of police officer applicants by law enforcement agencies
throughout the United States. Research indicates that since 1995, there has been an
increased use of the polygraph exam as a screening tool for law enforcement applicants in
the United States (Hughes, & Langworthy, 1995; Sanders, Hughes, & Langworthy, 1995;
U.S. Department of Justice, 2000; Reaves, 2010). Many federal and local law
enforcement agencies use the polygraph exam for criminal investigations as well as for
personnel selection (Kanable, 2010; Honts & Perry, 1992). Research indicates that
although the polygraph exam is used by law enforcement agencies in the screening
41
process of applicants, that use is not consistent, standardized, and not fully regulated
(Grubin & Madsen, 2005).
The lack of practice standardization in the use of the polygraph exam
administration for the police officer selection process is a challenge, and there is a lack of
effort among practitioners to develop a body of best practices for screening examinations
that could increase the reliability and validity of the polygraph exam when used as a
screening tool (Handler, Honts, Krapohl, Nelson, & Griffin, 2009). The American
Polygraph Association, established in 1966, is the largest professional association for
polygraphy in the world and has a code of ethics, standards of practice, and grievance
procedures, but non-American Polygraph Association accredited schools exist. There is
little regulation of polygraph examiners other than twenty of the states requiring
polygraph examiners to be registered with a state board or the American Polygraph
Association. Individuals do not have to be licensed or have particular training to
purchase a polygraph instrument and practice polygraphy privately. The consistency of
use, regulation, and standardization of use of the polygraph exam remains problematic,
especially when used as a screening tool by law enforcement agencies to screen police
officer applicants (Grubin & Madsen, 2005). Adopting more standardized approaches in
the administration and evaluation of polygraph exams will increase the reliability and
validity of this screening method which is a very important factor to consider since this
method is applied to the hiring process of individuals responsible for public safety
(Handler, Honts, Krapohl, Nelson, & Griffin, 2009).
42
Limitations
The current study provided additional information to the research on the law
enforcement screening process but was limited by a narrow focus on the ten largest law
enforcement agencies, a low response rate, whether or not the responses were truthful or
accurate, and a reluctance by the participants to divulge information. Not all of the
surveys were completed and returned, and some were returned incomplete. In addition to
the surveys, other methods were used to obtain information and data such as reading law
enforcement blogs, sending emails, reading websites and making telephone calls to law
enforcement agencies. The participants may not have been truthful in their responses to
the surveys, internet emails, and telephone calls, and the information and data obtained
from the law enforcement blogs and websites may not have been reliable.
Information and data obtained from internet websites may not have been reliable
since the websites may not be updated and maintained on a regular basis. The telephone
calls did not produce enough information and data because the participants were
apprehensive and not cooperative about divulging information about the law enforcement
screening process. Apprehension and non-cooperativeness seemed to be caused by the
participants not wanting to divulge information about the police screening process to
possible applicants. Even though the current study had limitations, the study provided
additional information to the available research concerning the law enforcement agency
applicant screening process and provided implications and future direction for further
investigation.
43
Future Research Recommendations
Future research should include more law enforcement agencies as participants in
order to obtain more information and data concerning the law enforcement screening
process with particular emphasis on the polygraph exam as a screening tool. Research
should investigate whether or not the use of the polygraph exam in the screening process
follows regulations and standardization. Sending surveys to multiple local state law
enforcement agencies across the United States, and not just the ten largest agencies,
would yield more information and data concerning the standardization of the screening
process that would be more generalizable and representative of the law enforcement
screening process in general. Other future research should include studies concerning the
screening process and how elements of the screening process, such as the polygraph
exam results, provide valid indicators of police officer success and job performance.
44
References
Aamodt, M. G., & Custer, H. (2006). Who can best catch a liar?: A meta-analysis of
individual differences in detecting deception. The Forensic Examiner, 15(1), 6-
11.
American Polygraph Association. (2012). Model policy for law enforcement/public-
service pre-employment polygraph screening examinations. Retrieved from
American Polygraph Association. http://www.polygraph.org/section/resources/
download-policies-and-acts.
American Polygraph Association. (2012). Model policy for the evaluation of examinee
suitability for polygraph testing. Retrieved from American Polygraph Association.
http://www.polygraph.org/section/resources/download-policies-and-acts.
Barland, G. H. (1985). Criminal investigation. Society, 22(6), 46.
Ben-Porath, Y. S., Fico, J. M., Hibler, N. S., Inwald, R., Kruml, J., & Roberts, M. R.
(2011, August). Assessing the psychological suitability of candidates for law
enforcement positions. The Police Chief, 78, 64-70.
Ben- Shakhar, G. Bar-Hillel, M. & Leiblich, I. (1986). Trial by polygraph: Scientific and
juridical issues in lie detection. Behavioral Sciences & The Law, 4(4), 459-479.
Ben-Shakhar, G., Lieblich, I., & Bar-Hillel, M. (1982). An evaluation of polygraphers’
judgments: A review from a decision theoretic perspective. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 67(6), 701-713.
Bersh P. J., (1969). A Validation study of polygraph examiner judgments. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 53(5), 399-403.
Bradley, M. T., Malik, F. J. & Cullen, M. C. (2011). Memory instructions, vocalization,
mock crimes, and concealed information tests with a polygraph. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 113(3), 840-858.
Brooks, J. (1985). Polygraph testing: Thoughts of a skeptical legislator. American
Psychologist, 40(3), 348-354.
Burch, A. M. (2012). Sheriffs’ offices, 2007 – statistical tables. Retrieved from U.S.
Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. http://www.bjs.gov/
content/pub/pdf/so07st.pdf
Candland, D. K., & Campbell, J. F. (1961). Lie detection: A measure of emotion.
Exploring Behavior: An introduction to psychology (pp. 113-119).
Chang-Bae, L. (2006). Psychological testing for recruit screening. TELEMASP Bulletin,
13(2), 1-7.
45
Cortina, J. M., Goldstein, N. B., Payne, S. C., Davison, H. K., Gilliland, S. W. (2000).
The incremental validity of interview scores over and above cognitive ability and
conscientiousness scores. Personal Psychology, 53(2), 325-351.
Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceutical, Inc., 509 US 579. (1993).
Faigman, D. L., Fienberg, S. E, & Stern, P.C. (2003). The limits of the polygraph. Issues
in Science and Technology, 20(1), 40-46.
Farber, F. J. (2011). Pre-Employment polygraph examinations of public safety applicants.
Retrieved from Americans for Effective Law Enforcement Monthly Law Journal.
Ford, E. B. (2006). Lie detection: Historical, neuropsychiatric and legal dimensions.
International Journal and Psychiatry, 29(3), 159-177.
Forrer, D., Mino, M., & Ehart, C.W. (2008). The effects of the hiring process on
employee termination: Concept and issues. Retrieved from The 2008 American
Public Administration Society Conference. http://professorforrer.com/Research_
Data/Papers/HiringProcessesvsTermination- Forrer_Mino-Ehart-ASPA2008.pdf
Ginton, A., Daie, N., Elaad, E., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1982). A method for evaluating the
use of the polygraph in a real-life situation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
67(2), 131-137.
Gordon, N. J. (2008, September). Today’s instrument for truth testing. The Police Chief,
75(9).
Gougler, M., Nelson, R., Handler, M., Kraphol, D., Shaw, P., & Bierman, L. (2011).
Excutive summary of the meta-analytic survey of criterion accuracy of validated
polygraph techniques. Retrieved from American Polygraph Association. http://
www.polygraph.org/files/excutive_summary_meta_analytic_survey_apa_2012.
pdf
Green, E. & Heilbrun, K. (2011). Psychology and the Legal System (7
th
ed.). Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth.
Grubin, D., & Madsen, L. (2005, June). Lie detection and the polygraph: A historical
review. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 16(2), 357-369.
Handler, M., Honts, C. R., Krapohl, D. J., Nelson, R., & Griffin, S. (2009). Integration of
pre-employment polygraph screening into the police selection process. Journal of
Police and Criminal Psychology, 24, 69-86.
Herron, D. J. (1986). Statutory restrictions on polygraph testing in employer-employee
relationships. Labor Law Journal, 37(9), 632-638.
46
Honts, C. R. (1996). Criterion development and validity of the control question test in
field application. The Journal of General Psychology, 123, 309-324.
Honts, C. R., & Amato, S. (2007). Automation of a screening polygraph test increases
accuracy. Psychology, Crime & Law, 13, 187-19.
Honts, C. R., Devitt, M. K., Winbush, M., & Kircher, J. C. (1996). Mental and physical
countermeasures reduce the accuracy of the concealed knowledge test.
Psychophysiology, 33(1), 84-92.
Honts, C. R., & Perry, M. V. (1992). Polygraph admissibility: Changes and challenges.
Law and Human Behavior, 16(3), 357-379.
Honts, C. R., Hodes, R. L., & Raskin, D. C. (1985). Effects of physical countermeasures
on the physiological detection of deception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70,
177-187.
Honts, C. R., Raskin, D., & Kircher, J. (1994). Mental and physical countermeasures
reduce the accuracy of polygraph tests. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 252-
259.
Horvath, F. (1985). Job screening. Society, 22(6), 43-46.
Horvath, F. S., & Reid, J. E. (1971). The Reliability of Polygraph Examiner Diagnosis of
truth and deception. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology and Police Science,
62(2), 276-281.
Hurd, S. N. (1985). Use of the polygraph in screening job applicants. American Business
Law Journal, 22(4), 529.
Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1997). The validity of the lie detector: Two surveys of
scientific opinion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82(3), 426-433.
Kanable, R. (2010). The truth surrounding lie detection technology. Law Enforcement
Technology, 37(8), 60-67.
Kircher, J. C., & Raskin, D. C. (1988). Human versus computerized evaluations of
polygraph data in a laboratory setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73(2), 291-
302.
Kircher, J. C., Horowitz, S. W., & Raskin, D. C. (1988). Meta-analysis of mock crime
studies of the control question polygraph technique. Law and Human Behavior,
12(1), 29-80.
47
Kurtz, J., M., & Wells, W, R,. (1989). The employee polygraph protection act: The end
of lie detector use in employment decisions? Journal of Small Business Manage,
27(4), 76-80.
Lamance, K. (2013). Admissability of polygraph tests in court. Retrieved from Legal
Match Law Library. http://www.legalmatch.com/law-library/article/admissability-
of-polygraph-tests-in-court.ht
Lewis, J. A., & Cuppari, M. (2009). The polygraph: The truth lies within. Journal of
Psychiatry & Law, 37, (1), 85-92.
Lie detector. (2013). Columbia Electronic Encyclopedia, 6
th
Edition, 1.
Lykken, D. T. (1985). Detecting deception. Society, 22(6), 34-39.
Lykken, D. T. (1974). Psychology and the lie detector industry. American Psychologist,
29(10), 725-739.
McDaniel, M. A., Whetzel, D. L., Schmidt, F. L., & Maurer, S. D. (1994). The validity of
employment interviews: A comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 79(4), 599-616.
Meijer, E. H., &Verschuere, B. (2010). The polygraph and the detection of deception,
Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 10(4), 325-328.
New rules govern polygraph tests. (1988). American Banker’s Association Banking
Journal, 80(7), 6-7.
Office of Technology Assessment (1983). Scientific validity of polygraph testing: A
research review and evaluation. Washington, DC: US Government Printing
Office.
Podlesny, J. A., & Raskin, D. C. (1977). Physiological measures and the detection of
deception. Psychological Bulletin, 84(4), 782-799.
Podlesny, J. A., & Truslow, C. M. (1993). Validity of an expanded- issue (modified
general question) polygraph technique in a simulated distributed- crime-roles
context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(5), 788-797.
Reaves, B. A. (2010). Local police departments, 2007. Retrieved from U.S. Department
of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. http://www.bjs.gov./content/pub/pdf/ pd07.
pdf
Sanders, B., Hughes, T., & Langworthy, R. (1995). Police officer recruitment and
selection: A survey of major police departments in the U.S. Police Forum, 5, 1-4.
48
Saxe, L., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1999). Admissibility of polygraph tests the application of
scientific standards post- daubert. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 5(1), 203-
223.
Saxe, L., Doutherty, D., & Cross, T. (1985). The validity of polygraph testing: Scientific
analysis and public controversy. American Psychologist, 40(3), 355-366.
Slavkovic, A. (2002). Evaluating polygraph data. Department of Statistics, Paper 186.
http://repository.cmu.edu/statistics/186.
Szucko, J. J., & Kleinmuntz, B. (1981). Statistical versus clinical lie detection. American
Psychologist, 36(5), 488-496.
Thurber, S. (1981) CPI variables in relation to the polygraph performance of police
officer candidates. Journal of Social Psychology, 113(1), 145-146.
United States v. Cordoba, 194 F.3d 1053 (1999).
United States v. Frye, 293 F 1013. (1924).
United States v. Schaeffer, 523 U.S. 303 (1998).
United States Congress, House Committee on Government Operations, Subcommittee on
Government Information and Foreign Operations. (1965). Use of polygraphs as
‘lie detectors’ by the federal government (House Report No. 198). Washington,
DC: US Government Printing Office.
US Congress. (1983). Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and
Evaluation A Technical Memorandum. Washington, D.C.: Office of Technology
Assessment, OTA-TM-H-15.
U.S. Department of Justice. (2000). Law enforcement management and administrative
statistics, 2000. Retrieved from Bureau of Justice Statistics. http://www.bjs.Gov/
content/pub/pdf/lemas
Warner, W. J. (2005). Polygraph testing. FBI Law Enforcement Bulletin. 74(4), 10-13.
Wells, G. L., & Lindsay, R. C. (1980). On estimating the diagnosticity of eyewitness
nonidentifications. Psychological Bulletin, 88(3), 776-784.
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2002). Eyewitness identification: Information gain from
incrimination and exonerating behaviors. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, 8(3), 155-167.
White, R. D., Jr. (2001). Ask me no questions, tell me no lies: Examining the uses and
misuses of the polygraph. Public Personnel Management, 30(4), 483-493.
49
Woska, W. (2006, October). Police officer recruitment: A public-sector crisis. The Police
Chief, 73(10).
Yeschke, C. L. (1965). Ethics and the polygraph examiner. Journal of Criminal Law,
Criminology & Police Science, 56(1), 109-112.
50
Appendix
Survey
The purpose of this study is to investigate the procedures that are used as part of
the law enforcement employee selection process. This research is being conducted by
Jessica Mark and Rebecca Mark of the Psychology Department, Rowan University, in
partial fulfillment of their M.A. degrees in School Psychology. Your participation in this
study will consist of answering the following questions and mailing the completed survey
in the enclosed, stamped, addressed envelope. There are no risks involved, and you are
free to withdraw your participation at any time without penalty. Your responses will be
kept anonymous. By taking this survey, you agree that any information obtained from
this study may be used in any way thought best for publication or education provided that
you are in no way identified, and your name is not used. Participation does not imply
employment with the State of New Jersey, Rowan University, the principal investigator,
or any other project facilitator. If you have any questions or problems concerning your
participation in this study, please contact Jessica Mark or Rebecca Mark at (856) 435-
0620 or their faculty advisor, Dr. Dihoff at (856) 256-4000 ext. 3783.
State:
________________________________________________________________________
Name of agency:
_______________________________________________________________
Size of agency: _______________________________________________________
Which of the following procedures do you use in the employee selection process? Check
all that apply and circle pre or post according to whether you use them as pre or post
conditional offer of employment:
_____Background investigation pre post
_____Criminal record check pre post
_____Driving record check pre post
_____Medical exam pre post
_____Drug test pre post
_____Physical agility test pre post
_____Credit history check pre post
_____Written aptitude test pre post
_____Personal interview pre post
51
_____Polygraph exam pre post
If you use the polygraph, please answer the following questions:
Who administers the polygraph exam?
Police Psychologist Chief of Police Other ____________
Who analyzes the polygraph exam results?
Police Psychologist Chief of Police Other ____________
Which polygraph machine is used? _____________________________________
Is the polygraph exam automated?
Yes No
How many polygraphs are administered to a single applicant?
1 2 More than 2
What is the scoring procedure of the polygraph exam?
Human Computerized Both
Describe the scoring procedure of the polygraph exam:
__________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
What is the selection/rejection rate for the polygraph exam?
__________________________________________________________________
Are the results of the polygraph exam disclosed to the applicant?
Yes No
52
_____ Psychological evaluation/Personality inventory pre post
If you use any type of psychological evaluation/personality inventory, please
answer the following questions:
Check all that apply:
_____ Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI, MMPI-2,
or MMPI-A)
_____California Personality Inventory (CPI)
_____Inwald Personality Inventory (IPI)
_____Neuroticism Extraversion Openness Personality Inventory (NEO-PI,
NEO-PI-R, or NEO-FFI)
_____Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI)
_____Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI)
_____16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF)
_____Clinical Analysis Questionnaire (CAQ)
_____Rorschach
_____Other: Name other tests_________________________________________
Who administers the psychological evaluations/personality inventories?
Police Psychologist Chief of Police Other__________
What is the selection/rejection rate for the psychological evaluations/personality
inventories?
__________________________________________________________________
Are the results of the psychological evaluations/personality inventories disclosed
to the applicant?
Yes No
53
Please number the following procedures in the order that you administer them during the
selection process. Leave blank if the procedure is not used:
_____Background investigation
_____Criminal record check
_____ Driving record check
_____Medical exam
_____Drug test
_____Physical agility test
_____Credit history test
_____Written aptitude test
_____Personal interview
_____Polygraph exam
_____Psychological evaluation/personality inventory
Do you allow appeals for any of the procedures of the selection process?
Yes No
Please describe the appeal process:
__________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________________
What is the overall selection/rejection rate of applicants? __________________________