Clemson University Clemson University
TigerPrints TigerPrints
All Theses Theses
12-2011
An Extension of Social Facilitation Theory to the Decision-Making An Extension of Social Facilitation Theory to the Decision-Making
Domain Domain
Allison Wallace
Clemson University
Follow this and additional works at: https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_theses
Part of the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Wallace, Allison, "An Extension of Social Facilitation Theory to the Decision-Making Domain" (2011).
All
Theses
. 1258.
https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/all_theses/1258
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses at TigerPrints. It has been accepted for
inclusion in All Theses by an authorized administrator of TigerPrints. For more information, please contact
AN EXTENSION OF SOCIAL FACILITATION THEORY
TO THE DECISION-MAKING DOMAIN
A Thesis
Presented to
the Graduate School of
Clemson University
In Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree
Master of Science
Applied Psychology
by
Allison L. Wallace
November 2011
Accepted by:
Dr. Fred S. Switzer, III, Committee Chair
Dr. Robin Kowalski
Dr. Cynthia Pury
ii
ABSTRACT
Social facilitation has traditionally been defined as the influence of the presence of
others on an individual’s task performance. Social presence has been shown to either
facilitate or impair performance based on various moderating variables, including the
more recent investigation of individual differences, but researchers have yet to extend
social facilitation theory to the domain of decision-making. This study evaluates the
effect of social presence on individual decision-making, using the personality traits of
extraversion, neuroticism, self-esteem, social anxiety, and trait anxiety as potential
moderating variables of this effect. We found that affiliative individuals, marked by high
extraversion and high self-esteem, demonstrated superior decision-making effectiveness
in the social presence condition of a decision-making task. Similarly, avoidant
individuals, marked by high neuroticism, high social anxiety, high trait anxiety, and low
self-esteem, demonstrated superior decision-making effectiveness in the alone condition
of a decision-making task. The influence of these findings on social facilitation theory
and the implications for real-world application are discussed.
Keywords: social facilitation, decision-making, personality
iii
DEDICATION
To my loving husband, Aaron, for his emotional support through all of my
endeavors.
iv
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
TITLE PAGE ....................................................................................................................... i
ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................................ii
DEDICATION ................................................................................................................... iii
LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. vi
LIST OF FIGURES .......................................................................................................... vii
CHAPTER
I. INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................. 1
Early Studies of Social Facilitation.............................................................. 2
Theories of Social Facilitation ..................................................................... 5
A New Direction .......................................................................................... 8
Personality as a Moderator ........................................................................... 9
A Novel Application .................................................................................. 12
II. METHOD ........................................................................................................ 16
Participants ................................................................................................. 16
Measures .................................................................................................... 16
Procedure ................................................................................................... 20
III. RESULTS ........................................................................................................ 22
Observed Distribution of Personality Traits .............................................. 22
Correlation between Personality Traits ...................................................... 23
Decision-Making Outcomes ...................................................................... 24
Comparison of Specific Personality Traits ................................................ 27
Sense of Evaluation.................................................................................... 28
IV. DISCUSSION .................................................................................................. 30
Limitations ....................................................................................................... 32
Future Research ............................................................................................... 32
v
Table of Contents (Continued) Page
V. CONCLUSION ................................................................................................ 35
APPENDICES
A: Personality Assessment Items ......................................................................... 37
B: Decision-Making Task Alternatives ............................................................... 40
REFERENCES .................................................................................................................. 42
vi
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
3.1 Bivariate Correlation Matrix for Personality Variables ....................................... 23
3.2 Hierarchical Linear Regression for Overall Effectiveness .................................. 24
3.3 Regression for Social Anxiety vs. Trait Anxiety ................................................. 28
vii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure
3.1 Condition * Propensity to Experience Social Facilitation Interaction ................. 26
1
CHAPTER ONE
INTRODUCTION
Individuals differ greatly in their reaction to the presence of other individuals
while completing a task. While engaged in an activity, an individual’s performance may
be either improved or impaired by the presence of others compared to when completing
the task alone. Recent literature has shown that the influence of social presence on
performance outcomes is dependent on the personality of the individuals involved. Thus,
through an understanding of the personality of individuals, we can determine optimal
settings for task performance. Individuals with personality traits that trigger negative
reactions to social presence will experience the best performance outcomes while
performing tasks alone, whereas individuals with personality traits that trigger positive
reactions to social presence will experience the best performance outcomes while
performing tasks in the presence of others. This theory, referred to as social facilitation
theory, has yet to be applied to the behavioral domain of decision-making. Through an
empirical investigation of the relationships among social presence, personality, and
decision-making, we can take the first, crucial step in understanding how to optimize
decision-making outcomes (e.g., efficiency, productivity, quality) based on the
personality of involved individuals.
Through the course of this paper, we first explain the lengthy evolution of social
facilitation theory, leading up to the recent discovery that personality is the crucial
moderator in the relationship between social presence and performance. Second, we
examine the current literature that assesses various personality traits as moderators,
2
isolating concepts which require further exploration. Third, we demonstrate the
importance of the application of social facilitation theory to the decision-making domain.
Finally, we present the results of a recent study which seeks to apply social facilitation
theory to an individual decision-making task, examining personality moderators of the
relationship between social presence and decision-making outcomes.
Early Studies of Social Facilitation
The concept of social facilitation was first introduced by Norman Triplett in the
late 1890s. Triplett (1898) observed that cyclists completed a race faster when they raced
with other cyclists compared to racing alone. Specifically, cyclists who competed against
each other produced the fastest times, cyclists who raced against a clock and also had the
assistance of a pacesetter produced the next fastest times, and cyclists who raced against
a clock without a pacesetter produced the slowest times. Upon this discovery, Triplett
then decided to test the speed with which children turned a fishing reel while performing
alone as compared to performing side-by-side with another child. Triplett found that
most children reeled the fishing rod faster while in the presence of another child who was
also reeling, as compared to children reeling the fishing rod alone. Triplett offered
several explanations for these findings. He argued that the presence of another reeler,
later termed a coactor, may stimulate a feeling of competitiveness which motivates the
target child to reel faster. Triplett also proposed that the presence of another person
performing the same activity might simply stimulate a thought to move faster. Later
researchers ventured to tease apart social facilitation effects from competition effects;
however, the importance of Triplett’s initial studies cannot be understated. These studies
3
demonstrated that people perform differently in the presence of others, even when they
are not interacting.
Allport (1920) coined the term social facilitation, which he defined as “an
increase in response merely from the sight or sound of others making the same
movement” (1924, p. 262). Allport’s studies were designed to minimize competition
effects, which were a potential confound in Triplett’s earlier studies. Allport’s first
experiments (1920) used two types of mental tasks in which he told participants to avoid
making comparisons with other people, stressing that the tasks were not competitive in
nature. In a word association task, participants were given a word with which they were
asked to free-associate, meaning that they should write down every word that came to
mind. In an argument-generation task, participants wrote down any argument they could
produce from a classic literature selection. For both tasks, participants performed both
alone and in groups. Allport found that people performing in groups made a higher
number of free-associations and generated a higher quantity of arguments from the
passage; however, the quality of the arguments was greater when they were created in the
alone condition.
Allport’s (1920) results were the first to suggest that social presence may have
varying effects depending on the type of task used. Allport also proposed that social
presence facilitates an idea or thought of movement and thus encourages movement.
Allport also noted that rivalry stimulates increased action, suggesting that over-rivalry,
distraction, and emotions could all lead to impaired performance (Aiello & Douthitt,
2001).
4
Triplett (1898) and Allport (1920) conceptualized social facilitation through
coaction, wherein individuals perform the same task in each other’s presence rather than
performing alone. Neither of these early researchers considered the potential effects that
an audience may have on individual performance, later referred to as the passive observer
paradigm (Dashiell, 1930) or audience effects (Zajonc, 1965). Dashiell (1930) proposed
that several forms of social presence exist, including the following: “(a) the mere
presence of observers, (b) the presence of others express attitudes about the individual
(later theories would refer to this as an evaluative audience), (c) the presence of
noncompeting coactors, and (d) the presence of competing cofactors” (Aiello & Douthitt,
p. 165). Dashiell’s studies compared the effects of these four different forms of social
presence and found that, in terms of speed when performing a variety of tasks, the
observed group was typically fastest and the group of competing coactors was typically
second fastest.
These early studies provided insight into the possible explanations for facilitation
and impairment effects resulting from the presence of others. However, these study
results were highly inconsistent, likely due to small sample sizes and poor control over
experimental conditions. Guerin (1993) points out that many of these early experiments
designed to compare performance of individuals influenced by social presence and
performance of individuals working alone often had an experimenter in the room during
the “alone” condition. While valid conclusions cannot be drawn from these early studies
due to these serious confounds, the studies made strong contributions to social facilitation
theory, raising many of the questions that still predominate today’s literature.
5
Theories of Social Facilitation
Numerous theories of social facilitation have been developed over the years. Five
of the main theories are briefly discussed below.
Drive.
Zajonc’s (1965) influential review represented a milestone in the development of
social facilitation theory. Zajonc argued that social presence facilitates performance of
simple, well-learned tasks and inhibits performance of complex, unlearned tasks. Zajonc
acknowledged the inconsistencies in previous studies where some researchers found that
performance is enhanced by the presence of others, whereas others found that
performance is impaired by the presence of others. Based on the Hull-Spence drive
model (Spence, 1956), Zajonc’s theory of social facilitation contended that an
individual’s drive levels automatically increase in the presence of others, and that the
increase in drive or arousal can either enhance performance of simple tasks or impair
performance of complex tasks. Drive theory posits that increased drive enhances the
release of dominant answers and inhibits the release of subordinate answers. On simple
tasks, the dominant response from an individual is usually correct, leading to
performance enhancement; on complex tasks, the dominant response is usually incorrect,
leading to performance impairment. Although other theories of social facilitation have
emerged and challenged Zajonc’s drive theory, Zajonc’s studies were crucial to the
evolution of social facilitation theory in that they introduced task complexity as a
moderator of social facilitation effects on performance, explaining the seemingly
inconsistent results of earlier studies.
6
Evaluation Apprehension.
Shortly after Zajonc’s original drive theory was proposed, Cottrell (1968, 1972)
asserted that the mere presence of others was not enough to increase drive and would not
necessarily lead to social facilitation effects. Cottrell argued that only when individuals
are concerned about how others might evaluate them would drive levels increase and lead
to either social facilitation or impairment effects. Cottrell’s theory of evaluation
apprehension is similar to Zajonc’s drive theory (1965) in that both theories propose
drive as a mediator between social presence and performance. However, Cottrell
proposed that this drive is learned as a result of past experience with social encounters.
In support of Cottrell’s theory, several studies have found that social facilitation only
occurs when observers had an evaluative presence (Cottrell, Wack, Sekerak, & Rittle,
1968; Paulus & Murdoch, 1971). Similarly, Bond (1982) proposed a self-presentational
theory in which social facilitation occurs due to the regulation and maintenance of a
public image of oneself and social impairment occurs due to embarrassment following a
loss of public respect.
Self-Awareness.
Two additional theories of social facilitation argue that social presence causes
individuals to focus their attention inwardly. Duval and Wicklund’s (1972; Wicklund &
Duval, 1971) objective self-awareness theory suggests that people tend to view
themselves from the perspective of their observers. The salience of any difference
between the current observed self and the individual’s ideal self produces an undesirable
state within that individual, motivating their future behavior to reconcile these
7
differences. In a simple task, this additional energy and motivation improves
performance, but in a complex task, the extra effort leads to impaired performance.
Similarly, Carver and Scheier (1981) proposed their control theory in which social
presence stimulates a process of comparing one’s own behavior against a behavioral
standard, stimulating a negative feedback loop in order to reduce any discrepancies with
the standard. In a simple task, outcomes are expected to be high, thus motivating
sustained effort. In a complex task, outcome expectancy decreases so that individuals
withdraw effort, having a negative impact on performance.
Uncertainty.
In a modification of his original drive theory, Zajonc (1980) suggested that social
facilitation effects evolve from the uncertainty that an individual feels in a social
environment. Guerin and Innes (1982; Guerin, 1983, 1993) further developed this idea in
their monitoring theory. Both the modified drive theory and the monitoring theory
propose that social situations pose a variety of threats to individuals, making it necessary
to maintain a high level of awareness in order to react quickly in case of a threat. This
high level of awareness causes an increase in arousal, which then leads to social
facilitation effects. Based on uncertainty theory, one would expect the strongest social
facilitation effects when an individual feels threatened, is unable to monitor the observer,
or is unfamiliar with the observer (Uziel, 2007).
Distraction.
The distraction-conflict theory (Baron, 1986; Baron, Moore, & Sanders, 1978;
Groff, Baron, & Moore, 1983; Sanders, Baron, & Moore, 1978) argues that social
8
presence creates a distraction for the individual, leading to a conflict of attention. Baron
(1986) suggested that this conflict of attention can have social and nonsocial causes, and
is most likely triggered by one of three specific conditions: (a) the distraction is
particularly interesting or difficult to ignore, (b) pressure exists to complete the task
accurately and quickly, and (c) paying attention to the task and the distraction
simultaneously is difficult or even impossible.
A New Direction
Despite the development of social facilitation theory through a large body of
research spanning over 100 years, “no single theory has emerged that can effectively and
parsimoniously account for this phenomenon” (Aiello & Douthitt, 2001, p. 163). Since
the existing theories previously discussed are not mutually exclusive, it is increasingly
likely that two or more theories co-occur as the best explanation for observed social
facilitation effects (Geen, 1989; Guerin, 1993; Uziel, 2007). More recently, social
facilitation research has begun to consider that two orientations toward social presence
exist. A negative orientation, reflected in the uncertainty, evaluation apprehension, and
distraction theories, dominates the social facilitation literature. Other explanations,
though less common, emphasize a positive orientation toward social presence, marked by
high levels of self-assurance and enthusiasm. Previous studies (Cox, 1966, 1968; Geen,
1977; Henchy & Glass, 1968; Seta & Seta, 1995) have attempted to manipulate the
relationships between the target individual and the observers by inducing a sense of
anxiety or self-assurance. Although there is a solid theoretical argument for the
distinction between positive and negative orientations toward social presence, Uziel
9
(2007) points out that experimental manipulations do not necessarily capture it. Uziel
instead proposed the assessment of personality traits, rather than the use of experimental
manipulation, to accurately capture these inherent positive and negative orientations
toward social presence.
Personality as a Moderator
Early researchers (Allport, 1924; Dashiell, 1935; Hollingsworth, 1935; Spence,
1956; Triplett, 1898) acknowledged the existence of individual differences in social
facilitation effects, yet they did not attempt to incorporate individual differences into their
various theories. After Zajonc’s (1965) emphasis on task complexity as the primary
moderator of social presence, later studies virtually ignored the already proven
moderation of individual differences related to social presence effects on performance.
Uziel (2007) notably points out that there is no theoretical basis for task complexity
classification, leading to the use of a vague categorization of simple versus complex
tasks. While the existence of a theoretical basis for task classification can be argued, the
issue is beyond the scope of this study. The crucial idea is that task complexity has more
recently been shown to be a poor moderator of social presence effects.
Bond and Titus (1983) conducted a meta-analysis of 241 social facilitation
studies, finding that social presence accounts for, on average, only a very small amount
of variance (approximately between 0.3 and 3.0%) in simple and complex task
performance. Bond and Titus also noted the rampantly inconsistent and often
contradictory results of studies, pointing to the obvious existence of moderators other
than task complexity. According to Uziel (2007), only 5-7% of social facilitation studies
10
have measured individual differences. These studies have focused on the measurement
of extraversion, trait anxiety, and self-esteem, showing that these three traits are strongly
associated with the positive and negative orientations toward social presence described
above. While extraversion and neuroticism are distinct from one another (Eysenck &
Eysenck, 1985), both are related to self-esteem, such that self-esteem has a moderate
positive correlation with extraversion and a moderate negative correlation with
neuroticism (Robins, Tracy, Trzesniewski, Potter, & Gosling, 2001).
The sociometer theory of self-esteem further demonstrates the relationship
between extraversion, anxiety, and self-esteem (Leary, 1999; Leary & Baumeister, 2000;
Leary & MacDonald, 2003; Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995). According to
sociometer theory, self-esteem serves as an indication of an individual’s interpersonal
appeal and perceived success. In social settings, extraverts tend to exhibit assertiveness,
optimism, and dominance. These behaviors lead to social approval and a sense of social
acceptance, which in turn is reflected by high levels of self-esteem. On the other hand,
neuroticism tends to lead to anxiety, depression, and shyness in social situations. These
socially undesirable behaviors lead to social rejection and a sense of social exclusion,
reflected by low levels of self-esteem. Therefore, positively oriented individuals (high
extraversion and high self-esteem) demonstrate a tendency toward self-assurance and
enthusiasm in both general and social environments, while negatively oriented
individuals (high neuroticism and low self-esteem) demonstrate a tendency toward
anxiety and apprehension in both general and social environments.
11
In a meta-analysis of 14 studies that measured individual differences, Uziel
(2007) found that social presence is associated with performance improvement in
positively oriented (high extraversion and high self-esteem) individuals and with
performance impairment in negatively oriented (high neuroticism and low self-esteem)
individuals. Most notably, Uziel found that personality is a more substantial moderator
of social presence effects than task complexity. Stein (2009) sought to examine Uziel’s
(2007) assertions regarding individual differences using a true experimental manipulation
rather than a meta-analysis. Stein measured extraversion, anxiety, and self-esteem
together, similar to Uziel’s use of negative and positive orientations, as well as separately
to tease apart the social facilitation effects. Stein argued that anxiety, instead of
neuroticism, was a more appropriate personality indicator to examine social presence
effects. Neuroticism and anxiety, though theoretically very similar, were both measured
in the current study to resolve the argument between Uziel and Stein regarding which is a
more appropriate measure in regard to social presence effects. Stein concluded that
personality traits indeed have an influence on whether individuals are facilitated or
impaired on tasks of varying complexity, but not in the same manner theorized by Uziel.
Stein found that individuals with high extraversion and high self-esteem were facilitated
in the alone condition compared with the social presence condition, and that individuals
high in anxiety did not change in performance between the absence and presence
conditions.
From Uziel (2007) and Stein (2009), it is apparent that individual differences,
specifically extraversion, self-esteem, neuroticism, and anxiety, are important moderators
12
of social presence effects on performance. However, given their conflicting results,
further research is needed to tease apart the true personality effects. It is important to
note that, although the terms positively oriented and negatively oriented accurately
capture these individual differences, these terms have been used as labels for multiple
other concepts in the field of psychology. In order to avoid confusion, we have chosen to
use the terms affiliative (high extraversion and high self-esteem) and avoidant (high
neuroticism, high anxiety, and low self-esteem) to represent these constructs.
Another unique contribution of this study involves clarification of the type of
anxiety that moderates social presence effects on performance. In the previous social
facilitation literature, studies which have analyzed individual differences have solely
employed the construct of trait anxiety. However, sociometer theory, which serves as the
theoretical foundation for explaining why individual differences matter in social
presence, utilizes the construct of social anxiety. Thus, this study attempts to bridge the
disconnect between theoretical basis and experimental investigation by measuring both
social anxiety and trait anxiety. Based on findings from previous studies, we expect to
find that trait anxiety is a moderator of social presence effects on performance, yet we
also expect to find that social anxiety is a stronger moderator of social presence effects on
performance, above and beyond trait anxiety.
A Novel Application
Social facilitation is generally described as “an influence of social presence on
individual performance” (Aiello & Douthitt, 2001, p. 173), an effect most often measured
through speed and accuracy in performance. Aiello and Douthitt noted the importance
13
for future studies to examine the effects of social presence on other qualitative behavioral
variations. However, every study from the lengthy history of social facilitation theory
has focused solely on individual performance on a variety of simple and complex tasks.
No study to date has attempted to extend social facilitation theory to the domain of
decision-making. Social presence may have profound effects on performance in
decision-making tasks, and its effects may differ from those found in the traditionally-
researched individual performance tasks. Understanding how social presence affects
decision-making is crucial to the field of organizational psychology because decision-
making is an integral component in business, often above and beyond individual task
performance. Both individual and group decision-making are critical behavioral domains
for most people; however, we often have little guidance regarding the optimization of
decision-making outcomes. By examining individual decision-making, we can take the
first step in understanding how personality affects decision-making outcomes in the
presence of others. This study therefore seeks to examine the effect of social presence on
individual decision-making.
Building upon the recent studies of individual differences as a moderator of social
presence on performance (Stein, 2009; Uziel, 2007), we expect that certain personality
variables will have a moderating effect on performance in decision-making. Based on
sociometer theory and the existence of affiliative and avoidant orientations toward social
presence, we know that individuals with high extraversion and high self-esteem are
predisposed to having an affiliative orientation toward social presence, and that
individuals with high neuroticism, high anxiety, and low-self esteem are predisposed to
14
having an avoidant orientation toward social presence. An affiliative orientation
indicates a tendency to approach social situations with a positive sense of challenge and
enthusiasm, and the ability to focus on the task at hand. On the other hand, an avoidant
orientation indicates a tendency to approach social situations with a sense of withdrawal,
marked by anxiety, depression, and distraction from the task at hand (Carver, Sutton, &
Scheier, 2000; Fiedler, 2001; Higgins, 1998; Isen, 1987; Mueller, 1992). Given these
tendencies and the type of decision-making task being used, it is expected that affiliative
individuals will experience performance facilitation and avoidant individuals will
experience performance impairment. Specifically, we expect to find that individuals who
are affiliative, measured by high extraversion and high self-esteem, will experience
performance facilitation in the social presence condition of an individual decision-making
task. We also expect to find that individuals who are avoidant, measured by high
neuroticism, high social anxiety, high trait anxiety, and low self-esteem, will experience
performance impairment in the social presence condition of an individual decision-
making task.
H
1
: Affiliative individuals will experience performance facilitation in the social
presence condition as compared to the alone condition, and avoidant individuals
will experience performance impairment in the social presence condition as
compared to the alone condition.
H
1A
: High extraversion is related to performance facilitation.
H
1B
: High self-esteem is related to performance facilitation.
H
1C
: High neuroticism is related to performance impairment.
15
H
1D
: High social anxiety is related to performance impairment.
H
1E
: High trait anxiety is related to performance impairment.
H
1F
: Low self-esteem is related to performance impairment.
Finally, we expect to find that individuals who have high social anxiety will experience
performance impairment, above and beyond the impairment related to high trait anxiety,
in the social presence condition of an individual decision-making task.
H
2
: Participants with high social anxiety will experience performance
impairment, above and beyond the impairment related to high trait anxiety.
16
CHAPTER TWO
METHOD
Participants
Participants for the study were 49 undergraduate students enrolled in introductory
psychology courses at Clemson University. Students received credit toward partial
fulfillment of a research-participation course requirement, as well as the chance to win a
$50 lottery based on performance on the decision-making task. Twenty-four participants
completed an individual decision-making task in a true alone condition with no
experimenter present, and the other twenty-five participants completed the same task in
the presence of an observer. The author served as the observer for all participants to
maintain consistency in the manipulation. Basic demographic information, including
age, gender, race, and educational status, were also collected. Participants were an
average of 19.4 years old, 65.3% female, and 85.7% Caucasian.
Measures
Personality.
Participants completed measures of extraversion, neuroticism, self-esteem, social
anxiety, and trait anxiety. All five scales are rated on a 1-5 Likert scale measuring the
extent to which participants felt that each item accurately described themselves, where
1 = very inaccurate and 5 = very accurate. For negatively scored items, items were
reverse-scored such that 1 = very accurate and 5 = very inaccurate. Scores for each
personality scale were summed to yield total scores for extraversion, neuroticism, self-
esteem, social anxiety, and trait anxiety (see Appendix A for list of items).
17
Extraversion.
Extraversion was assessed with a 20-item scale from the International Personality
Item Pool (IPIP; Goldberg et al., 2006), a highly reliable scale with a Cronbach’s α = .91.
Neuroticism.
Similarly, neuroticism was assessed with a 20-item scale from the International
Personality Item Pool (IPIP; Goldberg et al., 2006), a highly reliable scale with a
Cronbach’s α = .91.
Self-esteem.
Self-esteem was assessed with the 10-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (SES;
Rosenberg, 1965). Fleming and Courtney (1984) reported a Cronbach’s α = .88 for the
SES, and demonstrated a correlation of -.64 between the SES and anxiety.
Social anxiety.
Social anxiety was assessed with the 12-item Brief Fear of Negative Evaluation
(BFNE; Leary, 1983) scale, with a Cronbach’s α = .90. The BFNE is a brief version of
the Fear of Negative Evaluation (FNE; Watson & Friend, 1969) scale. Individuals who
score highly on this scale experience greater anxiety when in evaluative settings and
report being more bothered by the possibility of receiving a negative evaluation as
compared to individuals who score low on the scale (Friend & Gilbert, 1973; Goldfried &
Sobocinski, 1975; Leary, Barnes, & Griebel, 1986; Smith & Sarason, 1975).
Trait anxiety.
Trait anxiety was assessed with a 10-item scale from the International Personality
Item Pool (IPIP; Goldberg et al., 2006), with a Cronbach’s α = .83.
18
Decision-Making Task.
The decision-making task was developed by Straus and McGrath (1994). Straus
and McGrath pre-tested the task in a sample comparable to that of the current study to
ensure that the task was considered interesting and was of an appropriate level of
difficulty and length for the given population. The task session is 12 minutes in length.
Participants were asked to decide on appropriate disciplinary actions for a fabricated case
in which a graduate teaching assistant accepted a bribe from a star basketball player to
change the student’s exam grade. The goal of the task is to resolve the following five
issues related to the treatment of both the student-athlete and the teaching assistant: (1)
the student’s exam and course grade, (2) the student’s standing on the basketball team,
(3) the student’s academic standing, (4) the teaching assistant’s work standing as an
instructor, and (5) the teaching assistant’s academic standing.
Participants were instructed to decide upon one choice from a list of alternatives
for each of the five issues (see Appendix B for list of issues and alternatives). For
example, the alternatives for Issue 1 are “give him his original grade on the exam”, “give
him a failing grade on the exam”, and “give him a failing grade in the course” (Straus &
McGrath, 1994, p. 90). Participants were instructed to attempt to reconcile the competing
interests of the athletic department (wanting to maintain the player’s athletic eligibility),
the faculty (wanting to maintain the institution’s academic integrity), and the university
administration (wanting to satisfy both the athletic department and the faculty).
Straus and McGrath’s (1994) scoring system was used to evaluate the responses
to the decision-making task, yielding measures of overall task effectiveness, productivity,
19
and average quality. To obtain an overall measure of effectiveness, the scoring system
consisted of two sets of point values for the issue alternatives. The first point set reflects
the extent to which each alternative supports each faction’s (i.e., the athletic department,
the faculty, and the university administration) interests. For example, the alternative of
failing the student in the course on Issue 1 has a high point value for the faculty because
academic integrity is maintained, but has a low point value for the athletic department
because failing a course makes the student ineligible to play. The second set of point
values corresponds to the importance that each issue has for each faction’s interests. For
example, the issue of the student’s standing on the basketball team is very important to
the athletic department, while the instructor’s status as a student is of little concern. Each
individual was given three scores, one from each of the three factions’ point of views.
Each faction score was composed of the weighted point values for each faction, summed
over all five issues. The overall effectiveness score is thus a product of the three faction
scores. Using a multiplicative procedure ensured that the highest effectiveness scores
were awarded for solutions that involved the highest degree of balance among the three
factions’ interests, and that solutions which were highly favorable for one faction but
unfavorable for the other two factions received a lower overall effectiveness score.
No points were awarded for an unresolved issue so that participants were
rewarded for reaching a decision on all five issues, rather than leaving any issues
unresolved. Productivity was therefore simply measured by the total number of issues
resolved. Average quality was assessed by comparing the effectiveness scores among
20
individuals that resolved all of the issues, and analyzing the distribution of alternatives
chosen among individuals that resolved each specific issue.
Although there is no reliability or validity data available for the decision-making
task, Straus and McGrath’s (1994) article has been widely cited, appearing in 441
scholarly articles. Per the author’s personal correspondence with Dr. Straus, the task and
its scoring system are also frequently requested for use in research experiments.
Procedure
Participants were recruited by signing up for the study using the Human
Participation in Research (HPR) website, an online repository for research experiments
conducted at the university. In the lab, the experimenter conducted the informed consent
process with the participants. Participants first completed a basic demographic
questionnaire. Then participants completed a pre-task 72-item personality assessment,
which included scales for extraversion, neuroticism, self-esteem, social anxiety, and trait
anxiety. The order of the scales in the personality assessment was counterbalanced
across participants. Finally, participants completed the individual decision-making task.
All participants were reminded that the purpose of the task is to create the highest degree
of balance between the interests of the athletic department, the faculty, and the university
administration. In order to achieve this balance, participants were also instructed to
ignore their own preferences in the case. Individuals in the alone condition completed
the decision-making task while completely alone in a room. In the social presence
condition, the experimenter remained seated at a separate desk in the corner of the room
while the participants completed the task. The experimenter informed the participants
21
that “I am just sitting here observing and taking notes about the study process. I am not
evaluating your performance.” Participants answered one post-task question assessing
the extent to which they felt as if they were being evaluated, serving as an assessment of
whether participants’ sense of evaluation differed between the two conditions. Total
participation time was approximately 30 minutes.
22
CHAPTER THREE
RESULTS
Item scores were summed across scales to yield total scale scores for extraversion,
neuroticism, self-esteem, social anxiety, and trait anxiety. Because we hypothesize that
the propensity to experience social facilitation is the sum of multiple factors, we
combined the five scale scores to create an overall “propensity to experience social
facilitation” (PSF) score. Combining the five scale scores into one overall score also
contributes to increased statistical power for analyses. To create an overall measure of
these five personality traits that have an influence on the relationship between social
presence and decision-making, the scale scores for neuroticism, social anxiety, and trait
anxiety were first reverse-scored such that higher scores indicate less presence of the
given trait. The mean and standard deviation for each personality scale was then
calculated and used to standardize the five individual scale scores into z-scores for all
participants. Finally, the five z-scores were averaged to yield the PSF score. Thus, as
PSF increases (i.e., as extraversion and self-esteem increase, and as neuroticism, social
anxiety and trait anxiety decrease), an individual’s propensity for experiencing social
facilitation increases. We first visually inspected the spread of the personality traits to
determine if they were normally distributed.
Observed Distribution of Personality Traits
In order to draw meaningful conclusions, it is important to ensure that the
participant sample demonstrates a wide variety of scores for the five personality traits.
Neuroticism and trait anxiety scores were normally distributed with values observed at
23
both the high and low ends of the scales. Extraversion was also normally distributed,
although extremely low values were unobserved. Social anxiety scores were interestingly
bimodally, normally distributed with values observed at both the high and low ends of the
scale. Self-esteem was the only non-normally distributed personality scale,
demonstrating a significant negative skew. However, this finding is not unexpected
given that many studies (e.g., Blascovich & Tomaka, 1991) have noted that the
distribution of self-esteem scale scores among college students tends to be negatively
skewed (i.e., demonstrating range restriction).
Correlations between Personality Traits
We also calculated bivariate Pearson correlations between the five personality
traits (see Table 3.1). As expected, self-esteem was moderately positively correlated with
extraversion, r(47) = .495, p < .01, and moderately negatively correlated with
neuroticism, r(47) = -.595, p < .01. Neuroticism and trait anxiety were very strongly
positively correlated, r(47) = .918, p < .01, a finding that is later discussed.
Table 3.1. Bivariate Correlation Matrix for Personality Variables
Extraversion Neuroticism Self-Esteem Social Anxiety Trait Anxiety
Trait Anxiety -.369** .918** -.489** .567** 1
Social Anxiety -.437** .563** -.550** 1
Self-Esteem .495** -.595** 1
Neuroticism -.475** 1
Extraversion 1
Note. Correlation is significant at the .01 level (2-tailed).
24
Decision-Making Outcomes
Next, we analyzed the relationship between PSF, experimental condition, and the
interaction between PSF and condition as it relates to the three decision-making
outcomes—overall effectiveness, productivity, and average quality.
Overall Effectiveness.
We conducted a hierarchical linear regression analysis, testing the effect of
experimental condition (alone vs. social presence) and PSF on overall effectiveness in the
first step, and adding the interaction between condition and PSF in the second step (see
Table 3.2). Condition did not significantly predict overall effectiveness, t(45) = .098, p =
.923. PSF also did not significantly predict overall effectiveness, t(45) = -1.197, p =
.238. As hypothesized, the interaction between condition and PSF approached
significance, B = 1913.882, t(45) = 1.962, p = .056. The interaction between condition
and PSF explained an additional 7.7% proportion of the variance, beyond condition and
PSF alone, in overall effectiveness scores, R
2
= .079.
Table 3.2. Hierarchical Linear Regression for Overall Effectiveness
Variables Entered B Std. Error T Cumulative R
2
R
2
Step 1
Intercept 6335.114 567.113 .002 .002
Condition 76.058 794.111 .096
PSF 129.448 502.681 .258
Step 2
Intercept 6356.690 550.441 .080 .079
Condition 75.330 770.612 .098
PSF -824.482 688.779 -1.197
Condition * PSF 1913.882 975.616 1.962
Note.
p < .10
25
The interaction occurred in the hypothesized direction, illustrated in Figure 3.1.
Individuals with a high propensity to experience social facilitation (M + 1 SD; PSF = .80)
experienced performance facilitation in the social presence condition as compared to the
alone condition, such that overall effectiveness scores were much higher in the social
presence condition (M = 7310) than in the alone condition (M = 5698). Individuals with
a low propensity to experience social facilitation (M – 1 SD; PSF = -.80) experienced
performance impairment in the social presence condition as compared to the alone
condition, such that overall effectiveness scores were much lower in the social presence
condition (M = 5562) than in the alone condition (M = 7014). Individuals with an
average propensity to experience social facilitation (M; PSF = .0) experienced no change
in overall effectiveness scores between the alone and social presence conditions, such
that overall effectiveness scores in the social presence condition (M = 6432) did not
significantly differ from the alone condition (M = 6356). Interestingly, those individuals
with average PSF scores made less effective decisions as compared to both the high PSF
and low PSF individuals in their optimal decision-making contexts (i.e., in the social
presence and the alone condition, respectively).
26
Figure 3.1. Condition * Propensity to Experience Social Facilitation Interaction
Productivity and Average Quality.
Productivity is a simple measure of the number of issues that were resolved by
participants. Only 1 participant out of 49 failed to resolve all five issues; thus, a ceiling
effect eliminated productivity as a meaningful dependent variable.
The average quality of decisions was analyzed in two ways: (1) by comparing
effectiveness scores among only individuals that resolved all of the issues, and (2) by
analyzing the distribution of options chosen for each of the issues among individuals that
resolved that specific issue. Given these analytic methods developed by Straus and
McGrath (1994), an analysis of average quality is effectively rendered useless due to the
ceiling effect. However, we used the first method of analyzing average quality by
removing the one participant who did not resolve all five issues and conducting a simple
5000
5500
6000
6500
7000
7500
8000
8500
Alone Condition Social Presence Condition
Low Propensity to Experience Social Facilitation
Average Propensity to Experience Social Facilitation
High Propensity to Experience Social Facilitation
Overall Effectiveness
27
linear regression analysis testing the effect of experimental condition (alone vs. social
presence), PSF, and the interaction between condition and PSF. We found a slight
improvement in the significance of the interaction between condition and PSF as a
predictor of overall effectiveness, B = 2009.850, t(44) = 2.046, p = .047. The interaction
between condition and PSF explained an additional proportion of variance, beyond
condition and PSF alone, in overall effectiveness scores, R
2
= .087, which is slightly
higher than the model R
2
including all 49 participants.
Comparison of Specific Personality Traits
While our primary research question deals with the overall propensity to
experience social facilitation, we also wanted to conduct preliminary analyses regarding
the effects of certain specific personality factors.
Neuroticism vs. Trait Anxiety.
We previously explained that neuroticism and trait anxiety are theoretically very
similar, but we included both as predictors to resolve the argument between Uziel (2007)
and Stein (2009) regarding which is a more appropriate measure in regard to social
presence effects. Given that the correlation is r(47) = .918, p < .01, we are unable to test
whether neuroticism or trait anxiety is a stronger predictor of social presence effects
given the multi-collinearity between neuroticism and trait anxiety.
Social Anxiety vs. Trait Anxiety.
We ran two simple linear regression analyses to determine if social anxiety or trait
anxiety is a better predictor of overall decision-making effectiveness (see Table 3.3). It is
important to note that neuroticism was not included in either model due to its high
28
correlation with trait anxiety. The first regression analysis included extraversion, self-
esteem, and trait anxiety as predictors of overall effectiveness. Trait anxiety did not
significantly predict overall effectiveness, B = -61.154, t(45) = -.132, p = .895. The
model explained a small proportion of variance in overall effectiveness scores, R
2
= .038.
The second regression analysis included extraversion, self-esteem, and social anxiety as
predictors of overall effectiveness. Social anxiety approached significance as a predictor
of overall effectiveness, B = 831.470, t(45) = 1.755, p = .086. The model also explained
a larger proportion of variance in overall effectiveness scores than the previous model, R
2
= .099. Thus, social anxiety is a stronger predictor of overall effectiveness scores than
trait anxiety.
Table 3.3. Regression for Social Anxiety vs. Trait Anxiety
Variables Entered B Std. Error t R
2
Model 1
Intercept 6373.921 393.924 .038
Extraversion -573.201 464.636 -1.234
Self-Esteem 447.658 495.144 .904
Trait Anxiety -61.154 462.923 -.132
Model 2
Intercept 6373.920 381.168 .099
Extraversion -765.052 455.145 -1.681
Self-Esteem 55.006 490.273 .112
Social Anxiety 831.470 473.694 1.755
Note.
p < .10.
Sense of Evaluation
Finally, we conducted a simple comparison of means to determine if participants
in the social presence condition felt a greater sense of evaluation compared to participants
in the alone condition. Sense of evaluation in the social presence condition (M = 3.240,
29
SD = 1.091) did not significantly differ from sense of evaluation in the alone condition
(M = 3.208 SD = 1.250). Rather, participants’ sense of how much they were being
evaluated was related to participants’ social anxiety, r(49) = .277, p = .054. In the alone
condition, participants’ sense of how much they were being evaluated was moderately
correlated with social anxiety, r(24) = .462, p = .023. However, in the social presence
condition, there was no relationship between sense of evaluation and social anxiety, r(25)
= .079, p = .708. Interpretation of the meaningfulness of these split group correlations is
difficult given the low sample size within each group.
30
CHAPTER FOUR
DISCUSSION
We have demonstrated that personality moderates the relationship between social
presence and decision-making outcomes. Specifically, we established a propensity to
experience social facilitation (PSF) score, which increases as extraversion and self-
esteem increase and as neuroticism, social anxiety, and trait anxiety decrease.
Individuals with higher PSF scores made more effective decisions when making those
decisions in the presence of another person. Individuals with lower PSF scores made
more effective decisions when making those decisions without another person present.
Finally, individuals with average PSF scores (i.e., average scale scores on all of the
personality traits) made equally effective decisions when making those decisions in the
presence of another person or without another person present.
The level of decision-making effectiveness for those individuals with average PSF
scores was notably significantly lower than for both individuals with higher PSF scores in
the social presence condition and individuals with lower PSF scores in the alone
condition. Although individuals with average PSF scores performed equally well
whether in the presence of another person or alone, indicating that they were unaffected
by the experimental manipulation, those with both high PSF scores and low PSF scores
can reach more effective decisions when doing so in their optimal decision-making states
(i.e., in the presence of others for high PSF scores and alone for low PSF scores). This
finding suggests that we can optimize decision-making effectiveness for people who fall
at both the high and low ends of extraversion, neuroticism, self-esteem, social anxiety,
31
and trait anxiety by tailoring the context in which they make decisions; however, people
who are at typical levels in terms of personality cannot increase decision-making
performance by changing the situation in which decisions are made.
Participants demonstrated a very high level of decision-making productivity.
Since the decision-making task was originally structured as a group task by Straus and
McGrath (1994), it makes sense that productivity would vary more when analyzing
groups because of the introduction of multiple individual personalities and the potential
for disagreement. Thus, a measure of productivity proved to not be as meaningful in an
individual task given the absence of these complexities.
We next analyzed several issues regarding the specific personality traits that make
up the PSF score. We could not draw a conclusion regarding whether neuroticism or trait
anxiety is a stronger moderator of social presence effects in decision-making due to the
multi-collinearity between neuroticism and trait anxiety. It is likely that the discrepancies
between our findings and those of Uziel (2007) and Stein (2009) are due to variations in
experimental manipulation. However, from these results, we conclude that neuroticism
and trait anxiety are equally strong predictors of social presence effects. We confirmed
our hypothesis that social anxiety is a much more significant moderator of social
presence effects as compared to trait anxiety. Finally, we contributed to the social
facilitation literature by (1) including social anxiety as a predictor of social presence
effects, and (2) finding that social anxiety is a stronger predictor of social presence
effects than trait anxiety. From this point forward, researchers interested in social
32
facilitation should include social anxiety in their analysis, a decision which is supported
by the sociometer theory literature (rf. Leary et al., 1995) and the findings of this study.
Limitations
There were several limitations to the study which need to be addressed. Although
most of the personality variables comprising the PSF score were normally distributed,
self-esteem was significantly negatively skewed, which is expected in a population of
college students. Using the same manipulation and measurement with another sample
(e.g., employees of a large financial firm), we may find different personality
distributions, leading to results different from the current study. It is also feasible that
college students, given their younger age and lack of experience, may make less effective
decisions than the general population, so another sample may demonstrate a different
pattern of changes in decision-making effectiveness when making decisions either in the
presence of others or alone. However, the topic of the decision-making task was very
relevant to the experiences of college students.
The decision-making task required a considerable amount of social intelligence,
which may have stacked the deck in favor of significant findings related to social anxiety.
Finally, it is important to note that the sample size, especially when making comparisons
between the alone and social presence conditions, is relatively small.
Future Research
Given these limitations, future research should attempt to examine the same
hypothesized relationship in a variety of samples. The hypothesized relationship between
social presence, personality traits, and decision-making outcomes might be opposite of
33
the relationship found in the current study when other types of decision-making tasks are
used. We found that affiliative individuals make better decisions in the presence of
others; however, there are other types of tasks, such as those involving the detection of
danger or a more realistic, rather than optimistic, sense of one’s own abilities for which a
more negative mood may lead to better decision-making. Under certain circumstances, a
sense of optimism may lead to overconfidence or lack of caution, and if the task is such
that overconfidence or lack of caution are heavily penalized, individuals who are more
realistic may perform better on those tasks. Future studies could examine the
relationship between social presence, personality, and decision-making in tasks where
caution is negatively evaluated and where risk is valued. It is also plausible that real-
world tasks, rather than hypothetical tasks such as the one used in this study, may be of
higher complexity and involve higher stakes.
Another direction for future studies is to extend this theory to group decision-
making. The use of groups introduces several other complexities into the decision-
making environment, such as group cohesion, perceived similarity, and the introduction
of multiple, and most likely different, personality types. An interesting direction would
be to examine the same hypotheses in a group decision-making scenario, comparing
groups with homogeneous (i.e., affiliative and avoidant groups) and heterogeneous
personalities. Future research should also examine social presence effects in other types
of group tasks. McGrath (1984) suggested that group tasks can be classified into one of
four categories of basic processes, namely generate, choose, negotiate and execute tasks.
Decision-making tasks such as that used in the current study are a part of the choose
34
category, along with intellectual tasks. The generate category involves generating ideas
and plans through creativity and planning tasks, the execute category involves executing
performance tasks and resolving conflicts of power through competitive and psycho-
motor tasks, and the negotiate category involves resolving conflicts of viewpoint and
conflicts of interest through cognitive conflict and mixed-motive tasks. Future research
should examine social facilitation effects using these other types of group tasks.
35
CHAPTER FIVE
CONCLUSION
Since its introduction in 1898, social facilitation theory has focused solely on the
impact of social presence on performance in individual tasks. Given this study’s findings
of social facilitation effects in decision-making, it is necessary to revise this traditional
definition of social facilitation. Although task performance is an important component of
the everyday duties of employees in the corporate world, decision-making is an even
more crucial and complex component of employee responsibilities. Social presence has a
proven influence on decision-making effectiveness. Using personality as a moderator,
this theory implies that individuals who are affiliative experience benefits from social
presence while engaging in decision-making, while others who are avoidant suffer
detriments from social presence while making decisions. The goal of any business is to
enable employees and managers to make efficient, productive, high-quality decisions.
For those with high extraversion and high self-esteem, they may be optimal decision-
makers while under the supervision or observance of others. For those with high
neuroticism, high anxiety, and low self-esteem, they may be optimal decision-makers
while working alone. An understanding of employees’ personality can provide managers
and supervisors with the ability to tailor decision-making scenarios to obtain the most
effective outcome.
36
APPENDICES
37
Appendix A
Personality Assessment Items
Extraversion (20-item scale; Cronbach’s α = .91)
+ Scored: Feel comfortable around people.
Make friends easily.
Am skilled in handling social situations.
Am the life of the party.
Know how to captivate people.
Start conversations.
Warm up quickly to others.
Talk to a lot of different people at parties.
Don't mind being the center of attention.
Cheer people up.
– Scored: Have little to say.
Keep in the background.
Would describe my experiences as somewhat dull.
Don't like to draw attention to myself.
Don't talk a lot.
Avoid contact with others.
Am hard to get to know.
Retreat from others.
Find it difficult to approach others.
Keep others at a distance.
Neuroticism (20-item scale; Cronbach’s α = .91)
+ Scored: Often feel blue.
Dislike myself.
Am often down in the dumps.
Have frequent mood swings.
Panic easily.
Am filled with doubts about things.
Feel threatened easily.
Get stressed out easily.
Fear for the worst.
Worry about things.
– Scored: Seldom feel blue.
Feel comfortable with myself.
Rarely get irritated.
Am not easily bothered by things.
Am very pleased with myself.
38
Am relaxed most of the time.
Seldom get mad.
Am not easily frustrated.
Remain calm under pressure.
Rarely lose my composure.
Self-Esteem (10-item scale; Cronbach’s α = .88)
+ Scored: I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal basis with others.
I feel that I have a number of good qualities.
I am able to do things as well as most other people.
I take a positive attitude toward myself.
On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.
Scored: All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure.
I feel I do not have much to be proud of.
I wish I could have more respect for myself.
I certainly feel useless at times.
At times I think I am no good at all.
Social Anxiety (12-item scale; Cronbach’s α = .90)
+ Scored: I am frequently afraid of other people noticing my shortcomings.
I am afraid that others will not approve of me.
I am afraid that people will find fault with me.
When I am talking to someone, I worry about what they may be thinking
about me.
I am usually worried about what kind of impression I make.
Sometimes I think I am too concerned with what other people think of me.
I often worry that I will say or do the wrong things.
I worry about what people will think of me even when I know it doesn’t make
any difference.
Scored: I rarely worry about what kind of impression I am making on someone.
Other people’s opinions of me do not bother me.
If I know someone is judging me, it has little effect on me.
I am unconcerned even if I know people are forming an unfavorable
impression of me.
Trait Anxiety (10-item scale; Cronbach’s α = .83)
+ Scored:
Worry about things.
Fear for the worst.
Am afraid of many things.
Get stressed out easily.
Get caught up in my problems.
39
Scored:
Am not easily bothered by things.
Am relaxed most of the time.
Am not easily disturbed by events.
Don't worry about things that have already happened.
Adapt easily to new situations.
40
Appendix B
Decision-Making Task Alternatives
Issue 1: Jack’s grade in the course
1a. Give Jack his original grade on the exam (a D).
1b. Give Jack a failing grade on the exam.
1c. Give Jack a failing grade in the course.
Issue 2: Jack's status on the basketball team
2a. Make no change in Jack's basketball eligibility.
2b. Suspend Jack from the next basketball game.
2c. Suspend Jack from the basketball team for the rest of the season.
2d. Suspend Jack from the basketball team for an indefinite length of time and
require that he appeal to be reinstated.
2e. Kick Jack off the team.
Issue 3: Jack's status as a college student
3a. Make no change in Jack's college status.
3b. Give Jack a warning, stating that if he is involved in another incident
involving cheating in the future, he will be expelled.
3c. Suspend Jack from college (classes and athletics) for the rest of the semester.
3d. Suspend Jack from the college for an indefinite length of time and require
that he appeal for readmittance.
3e. Expel Jack from the college.
Issue 4: Tom's status as an instructor (Note: If Tom is restricted from teaching, he loses a
source of income that helps pay his way through graduate school.)
4a. Make no change in Tom's teaching status.
4b. Give Tom a reprimand to be placed in his permanent record, which will be
seen by potential employers after he is finished with school.
4c. Suspend Tom from teaching for the rest of the semester.
4d. Suspend Tom from teaching for an indefinite length of time and require that
he appeal to be reinstated.
4e. Do not allow Tom to teach again during his time remaining in graduate
school.
Issue 5: Tom's status as a graduate student
5a. Make no change in Tom's college status.
41
5b. Give Tom a warning, stating that if he is involved in another incident
involving cheating in the future, he will be expelled.
5c. Suspend Tom from the college for the rest of the semester.
5d. Suspend Tom from the college for an indefinite length of time and require
that he appeal for readmittance.
5e. Expel Tom from the college.
42
References
Aiello, J. R., & Douthitt, E. A. (2001). Social facilitation from Triplett to electronic
performance monitoring. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(3),
163-180.
Allport, F. H. (1920). The influence of the group upon association and thought. Journal
of Experimental Psychology, 3, 159-182.
Allport, F. H. (1924). Response to social stimulation in the group. In F.H. Allport (Ed.),
Social psychology (pp. 260-291). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: progress and problems. In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental and social psychology, 19, (pp. 1-40).
Baron, R. S., Moore, D., & Sanders, G. S. (1978). Distraction as a source of drive in
social facilitation research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36,
816-824.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1991). Measures of self-esteem. In J. R. Robinson, P. R.
Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of personality and social
psychological attitudes (pp. 115-160). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bond, F. C. (1982). Social facilitation: A self-presentational view. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 42, 1042-1050.
Bond, F. C., & Titus, L. J. (1983). Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies.
Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 265-292.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). The self-attention induced feedback loop and
social facilitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 545-568.
Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality:
Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
26(6), 741-751.
Cottrell, N. B. (1968). Performance in the presence of other human beings: mere presence
and affiliation effects. In E. C. Simmel, R. A. Hoppe, & G. A. Milton (Eds.),
Social facilitation and imitative behavior (pp. 91-110). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Cottrell, N. B. (1972). Social facilitation. In C. G. McClintock (Ed.), Experimental social
psychology (pp. 185-236). New York: Holt.
43
Cottrell, N. B., Wack, D. L., Sekerak, G. J., & Rittle, R. H. (1968) Social facilitation of
dominant responses by the presence of an audience and the mere presence of
others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 245-250.
Cox, F. N. (1966). Some effects of test anxiety and presence or absence of other persons
on boys’ performance on a repetitive motor task. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 3, 100-112.
Cox, F. N. (1968). Some relationship between test anxiety, presence or absence of other
persons and boys’ performance on a repetitive motor task. Journal of
Experimental Child Psychology, 6, 1-12.
Dashiell, J. F. (1930). An experimental analysis of some group effects. Journal of
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 25, 190-199.
Dashiell, J. F. (1935). Experimental studies of the influence of social situations on the
behavior of individual human adults. In C. Murchison (Ed.), A handbook of social
psychology (pp. 1097-1158). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York:
Academic Press.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences. New
York: Plenum Press.
Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P. Forgas
(Ed.), The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 163-185). Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fleming, J. S., & Courtney, B. E. (1984). The dimensionality of self-esteem: Some
results for a college sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,
404-421.
Friend, R., & Gilbert, J. (1973). Threat and fear of negative evaluation as determinants of
locus of social comparison. Journal of Personality, 41, 328–340.
Geen, R. G. (1977). Effects of anticipation of positive and negative outcomes on
audience anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45(4), 715-716.
Geen, R. G. (1989). Alternative conceptions of social facilitation. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.),
Psychology of group influence (2
nd
ed., pp. 15-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
44
Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R.,
& Gough, H. C. (2006). The international personality item pool and the future of
public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84-
96.
Goldfried, M., & Sobocinski, D. Effects of irrational beliefs on emotional arousal.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1975, 43, 504-510.
Groff, B. D., Baron, R. S., & Moore, D. L. (1983). Distraction, attentional conflict, and
drivelike behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 359-380.
Guerin, B. (1983). Social facilitation and social monitoring: A test of three models.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 22, 203-214.
Guerin, B. (1993). Social facilitation. Cambridge: University Press.
Guerin, B., & Innes, J. M. (1982). Social facilitation and social monitoring: A new look
at Zajonc’s mere presence hypothesis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 21,
7-18.
Henchy, T., & Glass, D. C. (1968). Evaluation apprehension and the social facilitation of
dominant responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 446-454.
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational
principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
30, pp. 1-46). New York: Academic Press.
Hollingsworth, H. L. (1935). The psychology of the audience. New York: American Book
Company.
Irwin, J. R., & McClelland, G. H. (2003). Negative consequences of dichotomizing
continuous predictor variables. Journal of Marketing Research, 40(3), 366-371.
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. In L.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 203-251). New
York: Academic Press.
Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. (2002). Are measures of self-
esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a
common core construct? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 693-
710.
Leary, M. R. (1983). A brief version of the fear of negative evaluation scale. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9(3), 371-375.
45
Leary, M. R. (1999). Making sense of self-esteem. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 8, 32-35.
Leary, M. R., Barnes, B. D., & Griebel, C. (1986). Cognitive, affective, and attributional
effects of potential threats to self-esteem. Journal of Social & Clinical
Psychology, 4, 461-474.
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem:
sociometer theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.). Advances in experimental social
psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 1-62). San Diego: Academic Press.
Leary, M. R., & MacDonald, G. (2003). Individual differences in self-esteem: a review
and theoretical integration. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of
self and identity (pp. 401-418). New York: Guilford Press.
Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an
interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 68, 518-530.
McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice Hall.
Mueller, J. H. (1992). Anxiety and performance. In A. P. Smith & D. M. Jones (Eds.),
Handbook of human performance (Vol. 3, pp. 127-160). London: Academic
Press.
Paulus, P. B., & Murdoch, P. (1971). Anticipated evaluation and audience presence in the
enhancement of dominant responses. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
7, 280-291.
Robins, R. W., Tracy, J. L., Trzesniewski, K., Potter, J., & Gosling, S. (2001).
Personality correlates of self-esteem: Age and sex differences. Journal of
Research in Personality, 35, 463-482.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Sanders, G. S, Baron, R. S., & Moore, D. L. (1978). Distraction and social comparison as
mediators of social facilitation effects. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 14, 291-303.
Seta, C. E., & Seta, J. J. (1995). When audience presence is enjoyable: The influences of
audience awareness of prior success on performance and task interest. Basic and
Applied Social Psychology, 16, 95-108.
46
Smith, R. E., & Saranson, I. G. (1975). Social anxiety and the evaluation of negative
interpersonal feedback. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 429.
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale University
Press.
Stein, L. M. (2009). Individual differences in social facilitation. Unpublished dissertation.
Straus, S. G., & McGrath, J. E. (1994). Does the medium matter? The interaction of task
type and technology on group performance and member reactions. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 79(1), 87-97.
Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. American
Journal of Psychology, 9, 507-533.
Uziel, L. (2007). Individual differences in the social facilitation effect: A review and
meta-analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 579-601.
Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 4, p. 448-457.
Wicklund, R. A., & Duval, S. (1971). Opinion change and performance facilitation as a
result of objective self-awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7,
319-342.
Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269-274.
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Compresence. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Psychology of group influence
(pp. 35-60). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.