Roger Williams University Roger Williams University
DOCS@RWU DOCS@RWU
Arts & Sciences Faculty Publications Arts and Sciences
2007
Effects of Expert Testimony and Interrogation Tactics on Effects of Expert Testimony and Interrogation Tactics on
Perceptions of Confessions Perceptions of Confessions
Morgan Moffa
Roger Williams University
Judith Platania
Roger Williams University
Follow this and additional works at: https://docs.rwu.edu/fcas_fp
Part of the Arts and Humanities Commons, and the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Effects of expert testimony and interrogation tactics on perceptions of confessions.
Psychological
Reports,
100(2), 563-570.
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Arts and Sciences at DOCS@RWU. It has been
accepted for inclusion in Arts & Sciences Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DOCS@RWU. For
more information, please contact [email protected].
Psychologzcal
Report\,
2007, 100, 563-570.
0
Psychological Reports 2007
EFFECTS OF EXPERT TESTIMONY AND INTERROGATION
TACTICS ON PERCEPTIONS
OF
CONFESSIONS
'
MORGAN
S.
MOFFA AND JUDITH PLATANIA
Roger Williams
Univev.rity
Summary.-Evidence obtained through the process of interrogation is frequently
undermined
by what can be perceived as overzealous interrogation tactics. Although
the majority of psychologically oriented tactics are
legally permissible, they nonetheless
contribute to innocent suspects confessing to crimes they did not commit. The pres-
ent study examined the effect of expert testimony and interrogation tactics on percep-
tions of a confession. 182 undergraduates read a transcript of a homicide trial that
varied based on interrogation tactic: implicit threat of punishment (maximization) or
leniency (minimization) and expert witness testimony (presence or absence of expert
testimony). Analysis indicated that the type of interrogation tactic used in obtaining
the confession affected participants' perceptions of the coerciveness of the interroga-
tion process.
The process of interrogation to elicit a confession is an essential process
in our system of jurisprudence. The conventional wisdom has been and con-
tinues in part to be that an innocent person would never confess to a crime
he did not commit (Kassin
&
Wrightsman, 1981). Such self-incrimination is
typically considered irrational (Colorado v. Connely, 1986) or perhaps an
attempt to gain notoriety, as in the infamous kidnapping case of the
Lind-
bergh baby in which 200 people falsely confessed (Bernstein, 2006). Not only
are confessions sometimes unrelated to guilt, at times they have been obtain-
ed through bargaining or negotiating with a suspect. Considering the variable
nature of the interrogation process, as many as 20% of all confessions are
recanted (Wrightsman
&
Fulero, 2005). Regardless of whether a confession is
voluntary or coerced, confession evidence can be so persuasive to a jury that
its presence during trial renders all other trial aspects meaningless
(McCor-
mick, 1972).
There are several legal protections that apply to confessions, some in-
volving case law and some evolving out of the United States Constitution.
The cause and remedy for obviously coerced confessions is illustrated in
Brown v. Mississippi (1936). As a result of interrogators' use of physical tor-
ture or "third-degree" tactics, the Brown court vacated the defendants' con-
viction. This decision was based on the Constitution's provision of due pro-
cess, as codified in the Fifth and Fourteenth Amendments. The Fifth
-
~-~~p-~~~p
'Address correspondence to Morgan Moffd c/o Judlth Platanla, Roger
Williams
Univerwy,
One Old Ferry Road, Brlstol, RI 02809, or e mall (m~moffa@~ahoo com and jpldtan~d@rwu
edu)
M.
S.
MOFFA
&
J.
PLATANIA
Amendment also protects every defendant from acting as a witness against
himself. No government agent can lawfully compel a suspect to make any in-
voluntary statement at any time during interrogations. In Miranda v. Arizona
(19661, the court interpreted and applied the Fifth Amendment to provide a
fundamental right to all suspects to remain silent. These safeguards exist to
protect the suspect's rights, as well as to ensure the validity of the confes-
sions.
Social science research in the area of confessions is necessary to evaluate
the psychological implications of legally permissible practices used in inter-
rogations (Royal
&
Schutt, 1976; O'Hara
&
O'Hara, 1980; Macdonald
&
Michaud, 1987; Walkley, 1987; Inbau, Reid, Buckley,
&
Jayne, 2001). One
term with much significance for social psychology in the evaluation of a
Mi-
randa waiver is "voluntariness." In order for a valid law enforcement appli-
cation of Miranda protections, a suspect's waiver must be "knowing, volun-
tary, and intelligent"
(Melton, Petrila, Poythress,
&
Slobogin, 1997, p. 158).
The court is required to evaluate the context of the waiver in terms of the
"totality of the circumstances" (Columbe v. Connecticut, 1961; Samaha,
2002, p. 385). The 1991 decision in Arizona v. Fulminante created a
per
se
rule for confessions and a limitation on the exclusionary rule of evidence. If
sufficient evidence existed to convict a defendant in addition to a disputed
confession, the confession itself was considered to be harmless error, and
thus admissible. Before this case, a coerced confession was grounds for auto-
matic reversal.
Suspects waive their Miranda rights at an alarming rate of approximate-
ly
80% (Grisso
&
Pomicter, 1977). Given this, researchers need to under-
stand the details of when Miranda rights are invoked and the value of exam-
ining the many factors that affect the interrogation process. According to
Leo
(1996b), 4 out of
5
suspects waived their rights and submitted to ques-
tioning. Kassin, Meissner, and
Norwick (2005) found college students per-
formed significantly better than police investigators when evaluating confes-
sions as either true or false. In a series of experiments, undergraduates and
detectives viewed or heard prisoners confess to either fabricated or factual
crimes. College students were more accurate than detectives in evaluating the
truthfulness of the confession. This study provides empirical support for ad-
dressing the consequences of waiving Miranda rights.
The use of interrogation tactics
by police is intended to lawfully commu-
nicate a threat (maximization) or a promise (minimization) to a suspect to
elicit a confession. Explicit threats and promises are not always avoided; the
nature of the confession and how it is obtained must be within certain legal
boundaries in order to be admissible at trial. The communication of a threat
or promise in the interrogation process lends itself to empirical investigation,
and therefore is one focus of the present investigation. Jurors' perceptions of
M.
S.
MOFFA
&
J.
PLATANIA
on their own perceptions of maximization and minimization as well as testi-
mony provided
by an expert regarding confessions when evaluating coer-
cion. In addition, verdict preference was examined as a function of each of
the independent variables. Finally, there is a question of the moderating ef-
fect (if any) of expert testimony on the dependent variables. In order to al-
low meaningful comparisons, a no-coercion condition was included as a con-
trol condition.
Hypothesis 1: Jurors' perceptions of "pressure on defendant to confess"
and "fairness of the police interrogation" would be a function of interroga-
tion tactic (maximization vs minimization) and the presence or absence of
testimony by an expert. Maximization would be associated with increased
ratings of pressure and decreased ratings of fairness. These effects should be
moderated by the presence or absence of testimony
by an expert.
Hypothesis 2: There would be a significant interaction of expert testi-
mony and interrogation tactic on jurors' decisions of voluntary vs coerced
confessions, and verdict.
Hypothesis
3: There would be a significant interaction of expert testi-
mony and interrogation tactic on perceptions of accuracy of the testimony
by the interrogator.
Participants
One hundred eighty-two undergraduates participated in this study as
part of a research requirement or for extra credit. Fifty-one percent were
women (n
=
92) and 49% were men (n
=
90). All participants were between
the ages of
18
and 24 years. Ninety-five percent were Euro-American, unmar-
ried, and reported no prior jury experience. All participants were treated in
accordance with
APA's ethical principles (2002). In all instances, participa-
tion took place during a class period, in a group setting.
Materials
Participants read a
10-page transcript of a homicide trial. After reading
the transcript,
they responded to
a
45-item questionnaire measuring demo-
graphics and attitudes toward the trial scenario. Seventeen items measuring
attitudes toward the trial scenario had internal consistency of Cronbach
a=
.92.
All judgments were made on a scale of 0: Not at all important/accurate,
etc. to
6:
Very important/accurate, etc. See Appendix (p. 570) for examples
of key attitudinal items. The entire study took 30 to 45 minutes to complete
-
'American Psychological Association. (2002) Ethical principles of
ps
chologists and code of
conduct. Retrieved January
8,
2007 from
http://~~~.apa.org/ethi~s/~odk~0022Pdf.
M.
S.
MOFFA
&
J.
PLATANIA
Hypothesis
2
was not supported. Results of separate log-linear analyses
found no significant associations between expert testimony and interrogation
tactic as independent variables and perceptions of confession (coerced vs not
coerced) or verdict (guilty vs not guilty) as dependent variables.
With respect to Hypothesis
3,
univariate analysis of variance indicated a
significant effect of interrogation tactic on jurors' perceptions of accuracy of
the interrogator's testimony
(F,
,i,=
6.07,
p
=
.003;
q2
=
.07). Scheffk's test of
multiple comparisons indicated Maximization
differed significantly from
Minimization
(p <
.01); Ms
=
3.21
and 4.07, respectively. Participants exposed
to Maximization interrogation tactics evaluated the detective's testimony as
significantly less accurate compared to those exposed to Minimization. No
significant Expert Testimony
x
Interrogation Tactic interaction was found,
and there was no significant main effect of Expert
Testimony. See Table 1
for significant main effects for interrogation tactic on the dependent vari-
ables.
TABLE
1
PERCEPTIONS
OF
PRESSURE
AND
ACCURACY
AS
A
FUNCTION
OF
INTERROGATION TACTIC
--
Perception Interrogation Tactic
Maximization Minimization Control
(n=61) (n
=
55)
(n
=
66)
Pressure of defendant to confess
5.10a 4.15~ 4.52"b
Accuracy of detective testimony
3.21' 4.07" 3.53""
Note.-Means in the same row that do not share superscripts differ at
p<
.05
using Scheffk's
test of multiple comparison.
DISCUSSION
Overall, partial support for the hypotheses was found. Jurors' percep-
tions of pressure were associated with interrogation tactic. In
general, maxi-
mization was perceived as more coercive than minimization. The results sup-
ported Kassin and McNall's hypothesis (1991) regarding jurors' assumption
that "scare tactic" interrogations are not necessary when there is the belief
that law enforcement officials possess tangible evidence of guilt. Participants'
ratings of pressure were consistent with Kassin and
McNall's functional
equivalence statement concerning the psychological effect and coercive
equivalence of maximization and minimization. In their study, minimization
was characterized as a promise of leniency and not as pressure
per
se.
The
present measures assessed minimization as pressure to confess, an evaluation
different from the definitional evaluation of "implied promise." The results
supported the finding that, although legally permissible, interrogator's use of
maximization and minimization was perceived
by jurors as psychologically
coercive.
The present
study opened the door to examining the role of specific
types of evidence on jury decisions. Open-ended responses to inquiries
con-
EFFECTS OF EXPERT TESTIMONY
569
cerning the most important factors in reaching a verdict pointed to the im-
portance of evidence either directly or indirectly,
i.e., "no murder weapon";
"no physical evidence"; and
"only circumstantial evidence of guilt." Partici-
pants consistently referred to the absence of this type of forensic evidence in
the trial transcript, suggesting a possible moderating effect of evidence on
decision-making. The inclusion and manipulation of testimony regarding fo-
rensic evidence may improve understanding the effects of testimony
by ex-
pert witnesses. Subsequent research on the effects of interrogations, confes-
sions, and expert testimony should examine their associations with other
types of forensic evidence.
Although the expert testimony condition alone did not show significant
effects, there is the possibility that it had some effect on decision-making.
Based on the present results, it seems that expert testimony
by itself is nec-
essary but not sufficient to modify jurors' perceptions. Conversely, percep-
tions of interrogator were not a function of the combined effects of expert
testimony and interrogation tactic, unexpectedly. Although a moderating ef-
fect was not found, there was a main effect of Interrogation Tactics on per-
ception of the accuracy of the interrogator. Participants exposed to maximi-
zation interrogation tactics evaluated the detective's testimony as less accu-
rate compared to those exposed to minimization. Forensic-type evidence
may have a moderating effect on perceptions of the interrogator's testimony
as well.
One limitation of the study was the written presentation of the stimulus
materials. Video stimulus materials are the most favorable experimental
method to approximate actual courtroom experience. Enhanced presentation
of expert testimony, either via video or
in vivo
exposure, would probably fa-
cilitate a more accurate and complex perceptual process for participants,
leading to more ecologically and externally valid results. In addition, it is im-
portant to stress the issue of generalizing from simulation-type research with
undergraduate students to the behavior of actual jurors. This is particularly
important considering how infrequently students serve as jurors compared
with community members. The value of this study, however, is the insight
offered into factors affecting perceptions of interrogation tactics in a confes-
sion scenario. Researchers should examine the relation between perceptions
and expectations of forensic-type evidence and expert testimony on jurors'
decision-making in criminal trials.
REFERENCES
Arizona
v.
Fulminantc,
499
U.S.
279 (1991).
BERNSTEIN,
L.
(2006)
The
case that
never
dies: the
Lindbergh kidnapping.
lournal of American
History,
92, 1477.1478.
Brown
Y.
MississLpp~
297
U.S.
278 (1936).
Colorado
v
Connely,
479
U.S.
157 (1986).
Colz~mbe
v.
Connecticut,
367
U.S.
568 (1961).
M.
S.
MOFFA
&
J. PLATANlA
COSTANZO, M. (2004)
P.sychology applied to law.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
GRISSO,
T.,
&
POMICTER, C. (1977) Interrogation of juveniles: an empirical study of procedures,
safeguards, and rights waiver.
Law and Human Behavior,
1, 321-339.
GLJOJONSS~N, G.
H.
(2003) Psycholo y brings justice: the science of forensic psychology.
Crim-
inal Behavior and Mental
~ealtt
13, 159-167.
INBAU,
F.
E.,
REID,
J.
F.,
BUCKLEY,
J.
P.,
&
JAYNE,
B.
(2001)
Criminal
interrogation^
and confes-
sions.
Baltimore,
MD:
Williams
&
Wilkins.
KASSIN, S. M. (1997)
The psychology of confession evidence.
American I'.sychologist,
52, 221-
122
LII.
KASSIN, 5. M. (2005)
On the psychology of confessions: does innocence put innocents at risk?
Amerzcan Psychologi.st,
60, 215-228.
KASSIN,
5.
M., &GUDJONSSON,
G.
H.
(2004) The psychology of confessions:
a
review of the lit-
erature and issues.
Psychological Science in the Public lnterelt,
7, 33-68.
KASSIN, S. M.,
&
MCNALL,
K.
(1991)
Police interrogations and confessions: communicating
promises and threats by pragmatic implication.
Law and Human Behavior,
15, 233-251.
KASSIN,
S.
M., MEISSNER,
C.
A.,
&NORWICK,
R.
J.
(2005)
"I'd know a false confession if 1 saw
one": a comparative study of college students and police investigators.
Law and Human
Behavzor,
29, 21 1-227.
KASSIN,
5.
M.,
&
WRIGHTSMAN,
L.
S.
(1981)
Coerced confessions, judicial instructions, and
mock juror verdicts.
Journal of
flp
lied Soczal Psychology,
11, 489-506.
1x0, R. (195'6a) Miranda's revenge: pice interrogation as a confidence game.
Law and 5ociety
Review,
50, 259-288.
LEO, R. (1996b) Inside the interrogation room.
The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology,
86, 266-303.
LEO,
R.
A,,
&OFSHE,
R.
J.
(1996)
The consequences of false confessions: deprivations of lib-
erty and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological interrogation.
The Journal of
Criminal Law and Criminology,
55, 429-496.
MACDONALD,
J.
M.,
&MICHAUD,
D.
L. (1987)
The confession: interrogation and criminal profiles
for police
offi'cerc-.
Denver,
CO:
Apache.
MCCORMICK,
C.
T.
(1972)
Handbook of the law of evidence.
(2nd ed.) St. Paul, MN: West.
MELTON, G.
B.,
PETRILA,
J.,
POYTHRESS,
N.
G., &SLOBOGIN,
C.
(1997)
Psychological evaluations
for the courts.
(2nd ed.) New York: Guilford.
Miranda v, Arizona,
384
U.S.
436 (1966).
O'HARA,
C.
E.,
&
O'HARA,
G.
L.
(1980)
Fundamentals of criminal z'nvestigatlon.
(5th ed.)
Springfield, IL: Charles
C.
Thomas.
ROYAL,
R.
F.,
&
SCHUTT, S.
R.
(1976)
The gentle art
of
intewzcwing and interrogatzon: a profes-
sional manual and guide.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Russ~~o, M.
B.,
MEISSNER,
C.
A,,
NARCI~ET,
F.
M.,
&
KASSIN,
S.
M. (2005) Investigating true
and false confessions within a novel experimental paradigm.
Psychological Science,
12,
481-486.
SAMAHA,
J.
(2002)
Crimznal procedure.
(5th ed.) Belmont,
CA:
Wadsworth.
WALKLEY,
-
1.
(1987)
Police intcrrogatzon: handbook for invcstigatorc.
London: Police Review
lJubl.
WRIGHTSMAN, L. S.,
&
FIJLERO, S. M. (2005)
Forensic p.rychology.
Belmont,
CA:
Thompson/
Wadsworth.
Accepted February
20,
2007
APPENDIX
1. Please rate the importance of the confession expert's testimony to your verdict.
2.
How
accurate was Detective Fuller's testimony?
3. Please rate the fairness of the
policc interrogation process described in the trial
4. How much pressure was the defendant under to confess?
Note.-
Judgments were made on 7-point scales (0: Not at all lmportant/accurate to 6: Very
important/accurate).