DOCUMENT RESUME
ED 283 152
CS 210 489
AUTHOR
Smith, Michael W.
TITLE
Reading and Teaching Irony in Poetry: Giving Short
People a Reason To Live.
PUB DATE
[85]
NOTE
52p.
PUB TYPE
Reports - Research/Technical (143) --
Tests/Evaluation Instruments (160)
EDRS PRICE
MF01/PC03 Plus Postage.
DESCRIPTORS
Comparative Analysis; Educational Research; *English
Instruction; High Schools; *Irony; Learning
Processes; *Metacognition; *Poetry; *Research
Methodology; Teaching Methods
IDENTIFIERS
Direct Instruction; Rasch Model
ABSTRACT
Because so little research has been conducted on
methods of teaching literature, two separate but related studies were
conducted to (1) examine what it means to understand stable irony,
and (2) to compare two methods, direct and tacit, of teaching stable
irony. A 36-item test, comprised of questions about 7 ironic poems
and 2 nonironic poems, calibrated for difficulty according to Rasch
analysis, was administered to 514 students. In addition, 4
experienced high school English teachers and 12 students were
interviewed and asked to respond to 2 other ironic poems. Results of
the written test showed that the ironic items were in general more
difficult to explicate than the nonironic items. The interviews
suggested that the students had difficulty detecting the presence of
irony. Four high school teachers and their classes participated in
the second study comparing the direct method of teaching irony, based
on metacognition and Booth's (1974) four steps of reconstructing
irony, and the tacit method, which gives students more exposure to
ironic poems. Control groups were given no specific instruction on
irony. Results showed that the direct and tacit methods achieved
a
statistically significant difference in comparison with the control
groups, but that there is little statistical significance between
direct and tacit methods. The study suggests that college bound high
school students need to be instructed in reading skills, to prevent
misreading, and researchers might look into the effectiveness of
small group work versus large group work. The test used in the study
is appended. (sC)
************************************************k**********************
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made
from the original document.
***********************************************************************
U.S. OEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
Office of Educational Research and Improvement
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
CENTER (ERIC)
*This document has been reproduced as
received from the person or organiration
originating it.
O Miner changes have been made to improve
reproductiOn duality
Points of view or opinions stated in this doCu-
ment do not necessarily represent official
OERI position or policy
READING AND TEACHING IRONY IN POETRY:
GIVING SHORT PEOPLE
A REASON TO LIVE
Michael W. Smith
4242 N. Kedvale
Chicago, Illinois 60641
Teacher:
Elk Grove High School
Elk Grove Village, Illinois
Degree:
University of Chicago
Phones:
vvtirk: 439-4800
home: 286-3799
"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS
MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
Michael W. Smith
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)."
BEST COPY
AVAILABLE
Squire and Applebee (1986) report that over one-hffif of the time
spent in English classes is spent studying literature. Unfortunately for
teachers, however, there is little research on teaching literature to guide
them as they devise this instruction.
In fact, Purves and Beach (1972) note that there are few studies
which consider classroom or curricular treatment. They note the need for:
... the investigation of what the teacher does.
"Teaching" literature, intervening in the natural
response processes of young people, seems to have
an e.ffect on them, on their cognitive performance,
on their attitudes, and perhaps on their interest
patterns. This sort of intervention seems to have
more effect than does the manipulation of the materiffi
thught or the structure and sequence of material.
The nature and effect of different kinds of intervention
need to be explored, particularly the relation between
the type of intervention and the kinds of outcomes that
are sought or measured. (p. 182)
Researchers have not heeded their cali.
In the 1984 and 1965 reviews
of studies in Research in the Teaching of English done by Dietrich and
Behms and by Durst and Marshffil, only one of the few studies on literature
considered a treatment, and that study was not experimenthl.
Cooper (1985) argues that one promising approach for ekmluating the
results of classroom literary study is to focus specificaliv on one
literary skill. This research follows Cooper's suggestion and focuses on
understhnding sthble irony in poetry in two distinct studies. The first
study considers what it means to understhnd sthble irony in poetry. The
second study compares two methods of teaching students to interpret
stable irony in poetry.
3
2
A Definition of Stable Irony
Any discussion of irony must begin with a definition, for there are
few, if any, literary terms that have meant so much to so many. Muecke
(1969) writes that:
[Irony's] forms and functions are so diverse
as to seem scarcely amenable to a single
definition: Anglo-Saxon understhtement,
Eighteenth-century raillery, Romantic Irony,
and schoolboy sarcasm are ell forms of irony;
Sophocles and Chaucer, Shakespeare and Kafka,
Swift and Thomas Mann are all ironists; for
Socrates irony was a standpoint, the governing
principle of his intellectual activity; to Quintilthn
irony was a figure in rhetoric; to Karl Solger
irony was the very principle of art; and to Cleanth
Brooks irony is, "the most general term we have for
the kind of qualification which the various elements
in a ccntext receive from the context." (p. 3)
To study irony effectively, then, one must narrow the focus.
In my
research I am concerned with what is, perhaps, the most basic form of
irony. Muecke calls it simple irony. He explains:
The more familiar kind of irony is Simple Irony,
in which an apparently or ostensibly true
s.thtement, serious question, valid assumption,
or legitimate expecthtion is corrected, invalidated,
or frustrated by the ironist's real meaning, by the
true sthte of affairs, or by what actually happens. (p. 23)
Booth (1974) calls simple irony "stable irony." He defines sthble
ironies by noting their chief features:
1. They are pll intended, deliberately created
by human beings to be heard or read and understood
with some precision by other human beings; they are
not mere openings, provided unconsciously, or accidenthl
4
3
sthtements &lowing the confirmed pursuer of ironies
to read them as reflections against the author.
2. They are all covert, intended to be reconstructed
with meanings different from those on the surface,
not merely overt sthtements that "It is ironic that ..."
or direct assertions that "things" are or "the universe"
is ironic.
3. They are &1 nevertheless stable or fixed, in the
sense that once the reconstruction of meaning has
been made, the reader is not then invited to undermine
it with further demolition and reconstruction.
4. They are &1 finite in application
The reconstructed
meanings are in some sense loc&, limited. (pp. 5-6)
Booth recognizes the wide variety of ironies, but he begins his
discussion with stable ironies, for understanding sthble ironies is
a
fundament& literary skin. He notes: "Every good reader must be,
among
other things, sensitive in detecting and reconstructing ironic meanings."
Kennedy (1976) corroborates the importance of this ability: "We had
best
be &ert for irony on the printed page, for if
we miss it, our interprethtion
of a poem may go wild." (p. 19)
Those who are able to detect and reconstruct ironic meanings
share a
unique literary experience. Booth explains: "...
we should marvel, in a
time when everyone t&ks so much about the breakdown of values
and the
widening of communication gaps, at the astonishing agreements
stable
ironies can produce among us." (p. 62) He explains further:
I spend a great deal a of my professional life
deploring "polar" thinking, reductive dichothrnies,
either-or disjunctions. And here ! find mgself
saying that only in strict polar decisions can
one
kind of reading be properly performed. On the
one
hand, some of the greatest intellectu& achievements
seem to come when we learn how to say both-and
not either-or, when we see that people and works
5
4
of art are too complex for simple true-false.
tests.
Yet here I am saying that some of
our most important
literary experiences are designed precisely to
demand
flat and absolute choices, saying that the sudden
plain irreducible "no" [rejecting the surface
meaning
of a text] of the first step in ironic
reconstruction is
one of our most precious literary moments. (pp. 128-129)
The Detection and Reconstruction of Stable
Ironies
Booth argues that stable ironies produce
astonishing agreements
among readers. If that is so, it seems reasonable to
presume that readers
must interpret stable ironies in similar
ways. Booth and Muecke agree
that the first step toward understanding is
to recognize that an author is
being ironic. They quote Quintilian, who
argues that irony
...
is made evident to the understanding either
by the delivery, the character of the speaker
or
the nature of the subject. For if
any of these
three is out of keeping with the words, it
at
once becomes clear that the intention of the
speaker is other than what he actually
says.
Muecke's analysis is quite similar. He
argues that the contradiction
between the surface meaning and the context
suggests irony. According to
Muecke, the context is made up of what
we know about the writer and the
subject, what the writer tells
us about himself or herself above the
pretended meaning, and finally, what
we are told by the style.
Booth's analysis is much more specific and,
consequently, more
useful. He argues that an author can signal irony through
the use of one or
more of five clues. The first is a straightforward warning in the
author's
own voice. He cites three basic ways authors can give these
warnings: in
titles, in epigraphs, and in other direct clues.
A second clue occurs when
6
5
an author has his or her speaker proclaim a known
error, perhaps the
misstatement of a popular expression,
an error in historical fact, or a
rejection of a conventional judgment. Though
Booth does not discuss them,
illogical expressions would also be subsumed
by this clue. A third clue is
the existence of conflicts within
a work. Booth cites The Rape of the
Lock:
Now lap-dogs give themselves the rousing
shake,
And sleepless lovers, just at twelve, awake.
Unless Pope is unaware of the contradiction (a
possibility we cannot
accept), he must be being ironic. A fourth
clue is a clash of style. Booth
explains that whenever the language of
a speaker is clearly not the same
as the language of the author, we must be alert for the
presence of irony.
For example, from the first sentence of The
Adventures of Huckleberry
Finn, we are aware of the distance between Twain
and Huck. Consequently,
we attend to Huck's words with some suspicion. Other examples of
this
clue would be understatements and exaggerations.
The final clue, Booth
explains, is a conflict of belief. That is, vlienever
the speaker espouses a
belief that the author could not endorse,
we percuive irony. This is the
clue that most clearly informs "A Modest Proposal."
While he does not
discuss it in A Rhetoric of Irony, Booth believes that
this clue also
includes behavior by a speaker that the author could
not endorse. (1985,
personal communication) In each case the reader
must bring to bear
standards from outside the text onto the world of the
text, proceeding
from the belief that the author does not hold alien values.
Suspecting the presence of irony is not the
same as reconstructing
meaning. Booth is unique among theorists in that he
tries to explain the
7
6
process of reconstruction, a process that other theorists
appear to believe
is natur&. Booth argues that reconstructing meaning involves
four steps:
rejecting the surface meaning, trying out alternative
meanings, applying
one's knowledge of
iior, aril selecting arnag alternatives:
In this case I find L.Lloth's arguments less
satisfying, for he does not
explain how the alternative meanings
are generated.
I agree that in "the
usual case of quick recognition" these meanings
come "flooding in." (p. 11)
However, since the meanings that flood in
are not random, something must
control them, and it has to be more than knowledge of the
author, for one
can understand the irony of unknown authors. I suggest that
once one
rejects the surface meaning, one must consider what
in the work (or
situation) is not under dispute. For example, in
Browning's "My Last
Duchess" we reject the Duke's assessment of his wife's death
on the basis
of a conflict of belief. Browning simply could not endorse the Duke's
behavior. However, in my experience of this poem, alternative
meanings
did not come flooding in. Instead I had to consider carefully
on what I
could base a reconstructed meaning. While the Duke's attitude
towards his
wife is suspect, I have no reason to believe that her
easy smile and joy in
life are his fabrications. While his defense of his actions
is suspicious, I
do not believe that he is lying or being ironic when he explains
that he
would not "stoop" to speak with her about her behavior.
I can &so clearly
understand his enormous pride in his title. Further, the
situation is not
under dispute; the Duke is talking to an emissary from
a count about a
proposed marriage. With these and other such facts in
hand, I apply my
knowledge of the world. The Duchess seems wonderful; I
would rejoice in
such a match. The Duke behaves like other self-centered
people I have
8
7
known, though, of course, none of my acquaintances have gone to such
extremes to satisfy wounded pride. People do not allude to illegal acts
unless they have a motive for so doing. The Duke is willing to expose his
guilt to make his expectations of the Count's daughter clear.
Putting it
all together, I believe that Browning must be criticizing this murderous
egotist. Now I check this meaning to see if it jibes with my understanding
of Browning. It does. Browning's monologues often reveal
a speaker who
is blind to his own vices.
To replace Booth's four steps of reconstruction, then, I offer four of
my own.
I believe that readers:
1. Reject the surface meaning
2. Decide what is not under dispute in the work
3. Apply their knowledge of the world to generate
a reconstructed meaning
and., if possible,
4. Check the reconstructed meaning against their
knowledge of the author.
Study I: A Consideration of What It Means to Understand !rony in Poetry
Of course, Booth's self-study is of little use to teachers if the
interpretive strategies that he.identifies are just idiosyncratic. This
study attempts to investigate Booth's theories empirically by considering
what it means to understand irony in poetry in two ways. First, it
attempts to define the variable through the analysis of test results.
Second, it attempts to understand the interpretive strategies experienced
and inexperienced readers use when they encounter stable irony in poetry.
Designing the Test
One of the major problems that plagues research in the social
sciences is the absence of effective instruments.
If we are to learn
9
8
anything about a variable or an individual,
we must have some objective
way to measure that variable or individual. Wright gives
an illustration.
(1986, personal communication) When
we read that a high jumper has
jumped seven feet, we never ask how it
was measured. We understand the
variable height because we
can measure it objectively. When we are
assessing the medal chances of a jumper who has
never performed in the
United States, we look at how high he has jumped.
We don't say, "Well, he
jumped 7 3" over in China. Lets see how he does with
our rulers." That
would be silly, but it's the sort of silliness that
confounds much social
science research. For example, researchers suggest
that IQ is an index of
intelligence, yet an individuals score
on an 10 test may vary markedly
depending on the test he or she takes. Unless
we believe that intelligence
fluctuates from day to day, and, of
course, we don't, we should recognize
that 10. tests don't give us reliable information about
how intelligent
individuals are or even what intelligence is.
An effective instrument is necessary f
or meaningful research results.
To consider what it means to understand irony
in poetry, I developed a
thirty-six item test. (See appendix.) The test makes
four statements
about each of nine poems and asks readers to
agree or- disagree with each
of the statements. The true/false format reflects the
belief that a
normative understanding of stable irony is possible.
Seven of the nine
poems contain irony. Some of the items on these ironic
poems make
statements about information that is not under dispute.
In all, the test
includes fifteen items that address non-ironic information
and twenty-one
items that address ironic information.
I piloted a first draft of this test with
seven graduate students in
1 0
9
English.
I &so asked them to write a justification for each of their
responses. Most of the variation in response was due to ambiguities in the
questions.
I then revised the test trying to eliminate the ambiguities.
Three experienced English teachers and
one graduate student in English
took this second draft of the test. They
were unanimous in their
responses.
I piloted this revised version of the test with twenty-eight
freshmen
in the honors track, nineteen freshme.n in the
average track, and a class of
sixteen juniors and seniors.
I did a Rasch an&ysis of the results to
ev&uate its effe.ctiveness as an instrument.
More specifically, Rasch analysis fit statistics enable test
designers
to see if their items are independent of each other and of other variabths.
In addition, Rasch an&ysis allows test designers to examines whether
their items are functioning as they intended. Wright and Stone (1979)
explain, "that a more able person should always have
a greater probability
of success on any item than a less able person." (p. 69) If this
is not the
case then the item does not measure ability &ong th
varthble. Not only
does Rasch anal yis &low one to examine the effective of each item
for the
entire group, it &so enables test designers to examine whether
each
individu& is using the items as they were intended. Wright and
Stone note
that, "... before we c.an use any person's
score as the basis for their
measure, we must determine whether or not their particular pattern of
responses is, in fact, consistent with our expecthtions." (p. 4) That is,
students must perform in a pattern that approximates a Guttman (1950)
sc&e. They must tend to get the easy questions correct and
then miss
most of the questions that are beyond their ability.
10
On the basis of the pilot results., 1
further revised the test. The
revised test was administered to the
five hundred and fourteen students
who were part of my second study. (Two
hundred and fifty-three students
took the test twice. Eight students
who took the pretest did not take the
posttest. The first administration of
the test included only seven
poems,
five of which contained irony. The
second administration included two
additional ironic poems.) Rasch analysis
of these results showed that the
revised test approximates objectivity
well enough to use it with
confidence as a measuring device.
Interpreting Test Results
Rasch analysis assigns questions and
individuals a value that locates
their position along a variable. This 6alibration
is reported in logits, the
log of the probability that
an individual with ability at the origin of the
scale will get a question right divided
by the probability that he
or she will
get the item wrong. The meaning of
a logit is always the same. An
advantage of 1.1 logits always
means that the student has a 75% chance to
answer an item correctly. Locating items along
a variable on a linear scale
gives meaning to the variable. As Wright
and Stone explain,
once a test's
items have been validated,
...
it becomes practical to turn
our attention to
a far more important activity, namely,
a critical
examination of the calibrated items
to see what
they imply about the possibility of
some variable
of useful generality. We want to find
out whether our
calibrated items spread out in
a way that shows a
coherent and meaningful direction. (p.E13)
12
11
TABLE 1: A CALIBRATION OF ITEM DIFFICULTIES
"Du lce et Decorum Est" 3
-1.82
"Sonnet 18" 1
-1.56
"Duke et Decorum Est" 1
-1.53
"Sonnet 18" 4
-1.23
"Sonnet 18" 2
-1.02
"An Unknown Citizen" 2
-.93
"Dulce et Decorum Est" 2
-.93
"'next to of course god america i" 3 (I)
-.59
"'next to of course godamerica i" 4 (I)
-.55
"Sonnet 130" 4 (I)
-.51
"Sonnet 130" 3
-.48
"'next to of course god arnerica i" 2 (1)
-.47
"Dead Bog" 1
-.43
"Boom!" 3
-.38
"Base Details" 3 (I)
-.38
"Duke et Decorum Est" 4
-.24
"Base Details"
1 (I)
-.14
"Base Details"
2
.25
"Sonnet 13" 3
.00
"Boom!" 2 (I)
.01
"Base Details" 4
.01
"Dead Boy" 3 (I)
23
"Cornrnunitie" 4 (I)
.31
"Sonnet 130 2 (I)
.35
"Boom!" 1 (I)
.62
"'next to of course god arnorica i" 1 (I)
.63
"Communitie" 3 (I )
.75
"Comrnunitie" 2 (I)
.76
"An Unknown Citizen" 3 (I)
.79
"Sonnet 130" 1 (I)
.84
"Dead Boy" 4 (I)
.89
"An Unknown Citizen" 1
.92
"An Unknown Citizen" 4 (I)
1.04
"Communitie" 1 (I)
1.15
"Dead Boy" 2 (I)
1.38
"Boom!" 4 (I)
2.26
13
12
Table 1 displays the item calibrations. The easiest items
have the
most negative values. The most difficult questions have the
highest
positive values. The mean error for these calibrations is .12.
I have
ordered the items from the easiest to the most difficult.
The I
in
parentheses indicates that the item is ironic. The 1-4
indicates to which
of the poem's four items the calibration refers. The
mean of all item
difficulties is 0.00.
The data suggest that, in general; the items that
were not
ironic were easier than those that
were. A t test comparing the means of
the ironic sterns with those items that
were not ironic yields a t statistic
of 4.37, well above the critical t of 3.61 (p=.001)
for thirty-four degrees
of freedom. This means that the chances
are less than one in a thousand
that the difference in means is the result of chance.
Table 2 conthins
summary statistics of various groups of items.
TABLE 2: SUMMARY STATISTICS OF ITEM DIFFICULTIES
Mean
s.d.
Ironic items
.45
.72
Non-ironic items
-.62
.73
Non-ironic items in
ironic poems
-.15
.56
Non-ironic items in
non-ironic poems
-1.04
.60
Of course, this is not to say that all poems that
are not ironic are
easier than ali of those that are. Synthx, imagery, and
many other
characteristics of poetry contribute to its difficulty. Indeed,
much
modern poetry celebrates ambiguity and deliberately defies understhnding.
In my selection of poems to include on the test I did not
choose any such
.14
13
poems. Since I have argued that authors use sthble irony intending it to be
understood, I selected non-ironic poems that are straightforward, so that I
could make reasonable comparisons.
Ironic items appear, in general, to be more difficult than non-ironic
items. However, the item calibrations establish that within the ironic
items there is substantial variations in difficulty. Initially, I theorized
that the difficulty of ironic items would depend on the nature of the poem,
its syntax, imagery, etc. However, I underestimated the importance of
another major factor, the belief the irony atthcks. Booth explains that
"Every reader will have the greatest difficulty in detecting irony that
mocks his own beliefs or characteristics." (p. 81)
I believed that Auden's "An Unknown Citizen" would be a relatively
easy poem because its syntax and vocabulary are relatively easy. However,
the final question, "The author believes that the reader should approve the
kind of life the citizen led," was the second most difficult on the pretest.
To check to see if students prior beliefs could explain this,
I asked
one class each of ninth, tenth, and eleventh graders who did not
participate in the study to respond to this statement: "Living
a
comfortable life without controversy is desirable." This statement is
one
of the ideas that Auden attacks in his poem. Nine students strongly agreed
with this statement. Thirty-eight agreed. Twenty-one disagreed, and
no
one strongly disagreed.
I also asked students to respond to this statement:
"In a war the officers who plan the strategy are the true heroes, not the
soldiers who carry it out."
Sassoon attacks this position in "Base
Details." Only one student strongly agreed, four agreed, thirty-five
disagreed, and twenty-eight strongly disagreed.
15
14
Assuming that the students in my study had similar views,
majority went into "An Unknown Citizen" holding a belief that is a subject
of Auden's irony. That could.explain why the question was more difficult
than I anticipated.
Again assuming that the students in my study held
similar beliefs, they were predisposed to be sympathetic to Sassoon's
position, and this predisposition may have contributed to making the
item relatively easy to understand.
Of course, the belief being attacked is not the only factor that
contributes to making irony difficult to understand. The final question to
Nemerov's "Boom!" was by far the most difficult on the test. This question
reads: "7
'7' author believes that if people are fortunate it is because God
is watchihy out for them." When I asked the ninth, tenth, and eleventh
grade students who did not participate in this study to respond to the
statement: "If people are fortunate it is because God is watching out for
them," four strongly agreed, thirty agreed, twenty-six disagreed, and eight
strongly disagreed.
Assuming that the students in my study held similar beliefs, slightly
more than half of them were predisposed to accept Nernerov's irony. Many
fewer were prepared to accept Auden's irony, yet the item on "Boom!" was
much more difficult than the item on "An Unknown Citizen." Nernerov's
allusions could explain the difficulty of the item on "Boom!" To understand
Nemerov's belief, one must understand the significance of Job and Demian
and Karnak and Nagasaki. While I gave a note to each of these allusions,
the note alone cannot explain all of the associations these allusions have
for an experienced reader.
It appears, then, that the belief that is the subject of the irony and
16
15
the intrinsic difficulty of the poem are the factors that make irony
difficult to understhnd.
The non-ironic items &so conthin
substhntial variation of
difficulty. The data support my theory that it is easier to understand
non-ironic items in poems that contain no irony than it is to understand
non-ironic items in poems that contain some irony. The mean level of
difficulty for the items in poems that featured no irony was -1.04. A t
test comparing this mean with the mean of non-ironic items in poems that
contain irony (-.15) results in a t statistic of 2.76, well above the critic&
t of 2.16 (p =.05) for thirteen degrees of freedom. This means that the
chances are less than five in a hundred that the difference in means is the
result of chance.
On balance, it appears that an understhnding of irony begins by
recognizing what is not ironic. As the ability to understhnd irony
increases., readers are better able to reconstruct ironic meanings in
increasingly difficult poems and to reconStruct ironic meanings that
ch&lenge their own beliefs and behaviors.
An An&ysis of Experienced and Inexperience.d Readers
Procedures
To examine more carefully the interpretive strategies readers use
when they encounter irony, I interviewed four skilled readers., each one of
them an experienced high school English teacher, and one student randomly
selected from each of the twelve classes that that participated in my
second study.
I interviewed the students before and after they received
their treatments. However, in this study I am reporting only the
pre-treatment interviews as I am interested in the students natural
17
16
responses. (I will report on the effect of the treatments on the interviews
in the second study.)
The two poems I used in the interviews are Sterling Brown's
"Southern Cop" and Robert Browning's "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister."
I
asked one qestion after reading the title and author. After each stanza I
asked one question that corresponds to one of the five clues that Booth
identifies.
I also asked the respondents to define irony. In each interview,
then, the subjects responded to twenty questions, six on "Southern Cop,"
eleVen on "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister," and three definitions.
After the interviews had been typed and coded, two raters rated each
of the twenty responses on the following four point scale:
1
Clearly shows no recognition of irony
2 Probably shows no recognition of irony
3 Probably shows some recognition of irony
4 Clearly shows a recognition of irony
The raters agreed or were only one point apart on over 92% of the
responses. On the twenty-one responses on which the raters scoring
differed by more than one point, a third rater scored the response.
I
eliminated the odd response.
If the third rating was between the other
two, I read the response to decide whether to omit the higher or lower
rating.
I used the scores of both raters to rate each response. Since only the
weakest responses would receive a one from both raters, I made that the
lowest rating point.
If one rater gave a response a one and the other rater
gave it a two, the response is probably stronger than one that received a
one from both raters. Therefore.. I gave that response a higher rating.
Using the same analysis, I created a seven point scale. That scale appears
18
17
below:
rater 1
rater 2
final score
1
1 1
1
2
2
2
2
3
2
3
4
3
3
5
4
3
6
4
4
7
I did a Rasch analysis of both the experts and the students' responses.
Results and Discussion
The Experts
The experts responded to all five of the clues that Booth identifies,
though clearly some were more suggestive than others. In general, the
position of the clue determined how suggestive it was. That is, the ironic
readings of the skilled readers evolved. They added each new clue to the
previous ones in order to generate their readings.
It is not surprising, then, that the straightforward warnings in the
titles were not very suggestive. Only one skilled reader clearly responded
to the negative connotations in the title "Southern Cop." (For the
discussion of both the experienced and inexperienced readers I will
consider only those responses that received a score of five or above as
clearly demonstrating a recognition of irony.) He noted that the title
"probably implies a negative feeling towards, let's say, abusive authority."
Also, only one skilled reader clearly responded to the explicit distancing
move that Browning makes in his title "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister."
As that reader explained:
Well, I know Browning often uses irony. There's often
an implied criticism of the speaker. The title doesn't do
19
18
much for me, though I see this as another dramatic
monologue.
The fact that it is a dramatic monologue does not
carry the weight for this
reader that it would for Booth, who argues that
any time the author thkes
pains to point out that he or she is not the speaker
we need to be alert for
the possibility of irony.
In fact, were Browning not the author, it appears
that the title would have carried little weight at all for this reader.
As a group, the experienced readers did not
appear to see a dramatic
monologue as a strong signal of the presence of irony. While the title
clearly establishes that the author was not the speaker, the experienced
readers withheld their judgment on the author's attitude to the speaker's
words. Perhaps because the experienced readers went into the
poem with
the understhnding that the speaker of the poem is not the
same as the
author, clues that highlight this distinction did not appear to significantly
affect their interpretations. This is not to say that other types of
straightforward warnings such as epigraphs would not
carry more force.
The speaker's proclamation of known errors was
a far more
significant signal of irony for the skilled readers.
In "Southern Cop" three
of the readers saw the speaker's faulty logic as an indictment. As
one
explained, "He's setting up a premise that on the one hand he wants
us to
accept because it seems logical; however, on closer inspection it's
obviously a fallacy." In "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister" the speaker's
error in the fifth sthnza was a telling clue. All of the readers clearly saw
it as an indication of irony. As one reader explained, "The author feels
that the speaker is a phony, and all of these phony rnanifesthtions of
the
fork crosswise and the three sips to honor the Trinity are embellishments
...The author is really cutting into the speaker." However, not all known
2 0
19
errors are the same. The experienced reader who had been
reading the
Browning poem as ironic from its title
on did not respond to the mistake
the speaker made in his prayer in the final
stanza. This shows that it is
difficult to generalize about the force of the
clues, for they are context
specific.
This point is also made clear by the experienced
readers' response to
clashes in style. All four readers received
a seven on the response to the
understatement in the third stanza of "Southern Cop."
As one explains,
"What the speaker says is an understatement;
it's not necessarily the
reaction one would expect, and because of that I think
there's a criticism
of the speaker implied by the author." Interestingly,
the experienced
readers responded to this clue more strongly than
they did to the conflict
of facts in the final stanza. However, only
one experienced reader
responded to the violence of the language in the first
stanza of Browning's
work. He explained, "I'm surprised by
some of the choice of words
...
and
just the vehemence of the language." The
other readers noted only that the
stanza establishes the extent of the speaker's
dislike of Brother Lawrence.
Their response to this stanza evidences their
caution in jumping into an
ironic reading. The difference in
response to these clues suggests the
extent to which the position of
a clue in a text determines its weight. In
'Southern Cop" the clash of style was the clinching piece
of evidence. In
"Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister" the clash of style
became significant
after the readers understood the irony through the other
clues.
The conflicts of fact were also important clues. Three
of the
experienced readers clearly recognized that the final stanza
of "Southern
Cop" indicated that the poem is ironic. As
one experienced reader
21
20
commented: "[Ty has] hod on unfortunate
experience in accidentally
shooting the man, but his tragedy isn't nearly
as great as that of the dead
Negro." However, one experienced reader
did not perceive this stanza as a
conflict of facts. Rather he saw it
as "a pretty clear shift of the poet's
description of Ty
... a more explicitly sympcithetic look at him for being
so stupid and ignorant." Three of the experienced readers
also clearly
recognized the conflict of facts in the fourth
stanza of "Soliloquy of the
Spanish Cloister" as a signal of irony. One
reader noted, "I think I'm
beginning to feel an attitude of hypocrisy here.
He speaks of the women in
very sensuous terms himself." Another simply stated, "The
author feels
the speaker is guilty of lust also."
The experienced readers seemed
more reluctant to respond to the
conflicts of belief than they did the other clues.
Even Ty Kendrick's
unjustified shooting of the Negro
was not enough to commit two of the
readers to an ironic interpretation. One reader
explained that she would
"have to wait and find out" about the author's
attitude toward these
events. Another explained that the validity of
the excuses "are the
questions I'm asking myself at this point." The
same hesistance marked
many of the responses to "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister."
On the third
stanza one reader commented, "The speaker is
very gleeful at any
perceived flaws in Brother Lawrence, and it's not
apparent yet whether
that is a criticism of the speaker." Another
says, "He seems so filled with
hate and sour grapes towards [Brother Lawrence],
you have to be a little
suspicious of his ire.
I want to know more at this point." By
the seventh
and eighth stanzas, though, all of the experienced
readers clearly
recognized that the speaker's actions were
a signal of irony. Indeed all
22
21
four readers received a seven for their responses to stanzas seven and
eight.
These responses indicate that the experienced readers saw
interpretation as an evolving process. They made hypotheses on the basis
of the clues, but wanted to wait to commit themselves to the hypotheses.
In fact, all four of the readers alluded to the dynamic aspect of
interpretation. One said about the second stanza that, "I can't tell if
there's sympathy for the speaker or not... I'm inclined to think so, but I'm
going to hold off judgment on that." Three of the readers made the point
clear in their response to the the seventh stanza.
One said, "By this time
we know that we've got a really spiteful, vindictive monk on our hands."
Another said, "It seems each stanza gets worse and worse in what the
speaker wants to do to Brother Lawrence." A third noted: "We're definitely
moving into an interpretation that revolves around the narrator's being
petty ..."
In each case it is clear that an interpretation developed through
the course of the poem. The conflicts in beliefs aided in that development,
but by itself a conflict of belief tended not to be enough to commit the
experienced reader to an ironic interpretation.
It is notable, however, that once the experienced readers perceived
the irony within the stanzas, they offered an ironic interpretation of the
whole poem. They did not fluctuate between ironic and non-ironic
readings. This behavior was only one of those that distinguished them
from the inexperienced readers.
The Inexperienced Readers
As I explained above. I analyzed the pre-treatment interview of one
student from each of the twelve classes that participated in the study.
23
22
Unlike the experienced readers who had
a firm grasp of the concept of
irony, the students I interviewed
seemed unfamiliar with irony. Before
the instruction, ten of the students
were unable to define irony. One
thought it might mean "to be
on a ship with pirates." Another thought it
might mean "kind of tough...bars of steel
or something." The two students
who were able to define irony
gave a definition of situational irony. None
of the students gave a definition that would
include stable irony. Readers
may not recognize stable irony if they are unaware that it
is an option that
writers exercise.
It is not surprising, then, that the
inexperienced readers
were much less responsive to the clues than the experienced
readers. In
fact, in seven of the pre-treatment interviews,
students did not make a
single response that received a final rating
of five or above.
None of the students perceived the straightforward
warning in the
titles. Those students who discussed the title
at all tended to see it as a
means to establish a setting. On "Southern Cop"
a typical response was
"Southern police, that's what I get...maybe
something racial, I'm not sure."
On "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister" only six
students made any inference
at all. A typical response of those who did
was, "Maybe it means this
Spanish monastery where this guy makes
a speech."
The interviews contained three examples of known
error proclaimed.
On the second stanza of "Southern Cop" and the fifth
stanza of "Soliloquy
of the Spanish Cloister" only one of the twelve
r6zponses clearly
recognized the proclamation of a known error
as a signal of irony. On the
ninth stanza of "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister" two
students responded
to this clue. The student who recognized the known
error proclaimed in
the second stanza of "Southern Cop" commented, "Makes
the Negro look
24
23
like he's innocent. Just 'cause he
ran doesn't mean he should have shot
him and Ty was looking for
a basic chance to make himself look better."
This student realized Brown's intention
in directing our sympathy to the
Negro. A far more common
response was one that confused the speaker and
the author. For example, one student reponded,
"The person who wrote it
might be prejudice against black people,
'cause he thinks he was
dangerous." lt is clear that this student rejects
the speaker's
characterization, yet she does not recognize that the
author shares her
view. Several students volunteered that they
thought that Ty was clearly
in the wrong, but they, too, gave
no indication that the author intended
that effect.
In "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister" most simply
paraphrased the action of the stanza. A typical
response to the fifth
stanza was, "The speaker's trying to show that he's
a better person than
the brother."
There was a similar pattern of
responses to the conflict of facts in
the two poems. Only one response clearly recognized
the conflict of facts
as a signal of irony in the fourth stanza of "Southern Cop." No
students
recognized the conflict of facts in the fourth
stanza of "Soliloquy of the
Spanish Cloister." The strongest response
on the final stanza of "Southern
Cop" explained that, "He deserves worse,
or whatever...again it seems
like he's criticizing him, or sarcasm... 'Let
us pity Ty'--we should not
pity Ty." On the other hand, the majority of
students once again tended to
paraphrase. As one explained, "Ty feels bad 'cause
he's standing there
wondering whether he's made the right choice and
now he knows he didn't,
and now the Negro's sitting there complaining to Ty
or making Ty feel bad."
This same tendency to paraphrase marked the
students' responses to the
25
24
fourth stanza of "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister." One typical
response
notes that, "The speaker thinks... or the other
guys in the monastery think
that he's lusting after women and everything and that he's
not worthy for
the church..."
The majority of the students also did not clearly recognize the
clashes of style in the poems as a clue to irony. Only
one of the twelve
responses clearly noted the irony of the understatement in sthnza three of
"Southern Cop." That student explained that, "He's criticizing--or
not
criticizing, but sort of sarcastic about Ty, using 'unfortunate
hich, you
know... it's unfortunate for the Negro, and the
use is a South-...
;e." A
far more characteristic response argued that, "He's kind of
prejudice--the
speaker--because he's trying to condone what Ty did..." Once
again the
respondent disagreed with the speaker's assessment of the situation
yet
failed to understhnd that Brown shared that disagreement.
The most compelling clue for the students was the conflict of
belief
in the eighth sthnza of "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister."
In fact, the
measure of this item was more than two standard errors lower than the
measure of the next easiest item. This is a sthtistically significant
difference. Four responses clearly recognized the irony of this sthnza.
As
one student explains, "The author thinks that the speaker is kind of corrupt
too, because he has this novel, and maybe Brother Lawerence is
not as bad
as he says." Several students realized that it was odd that the speaker had
such a novel, but they seemed unable to give an ironic interprethtion.
One
noted, "First he was saying Brother Lawrence was bad, and
then he's saying
he has this novel, and this novel is like bad,
so
...
I dunno."
In general,
though, this clue was the most evocative for two
reasons. First, the other
2 6
25
clues in the previous stanzas may have aided students in realizing the
irony of this stEnza. Second, the irony of the eighth stanza may be
apparent in a paraphrase or plot summary of the stanza, which, as I have
explained above, is a primary move for many of the inexperienced readers.
Realizing the behavior of the speaker does not require the attention to the
details of the poem as do the other clues. The inexperienced readers did
not respond as well to other conflicts of beliefs. Only one student
recognized the conflict of belief in the first stanza of "Southern Cop" and
the seventh stanza of "Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister." No students
recognized the conflict of belief in the second and sixth stanzas of
"Soliloquy of the Spanish Cloister."
Even when the inexperienced readers recognized irony within the
stanza, they did not always use their realization in their overall reading of
the poem. In fact, of the five s...idents who clearly recognized irony within
at least one of the poems, only one maintained ell ironic reading. For
example, consider the student whose response to the eighth stanza of
"Soliloquy of the E.., 3nish Cloister" I cited above. To my question on the
final stanza, he noted, "it makes the author think that the speaker talks
about Satan i,
his prayer, and he could be bad, too, and Brother Lawrence
would not be as bad." This student appears to realize that the speaker is
the target of Browning's poem. However, when I asked him to explain
BruNning's purpose in writing the poem, he said, "It illustrates why he
hatc
Brother LLwrence." This student appeared not to put the parts
together into a ,3herent whole. He was not alone in this tendency. The
transcripts reveal that many students appeared to see the stanzas as
discrete rather than as parts of a unified whole.
26
In general, the experienced readers made use of all of the clues that
Booth cited, though the straightforward warning contained in each
title
had little impact on most of their readings. For the experienced
readers
the interpretation of irony was an evolving
process. The clues that had the
most impact were those found late in the poem. These clues provided the
final pieces of evidence that caused the experienced readers to commit
to
an ironic reading. The experienced readers were cautious about making
such a commitment, especially when they had to apply standards from
outside the poem, as they would when they recognized
a conflict of belief.
However, once they made such a commitment they did not
waver from it.
The inexperienced readers, on the other hand, tended not to recognize
the clues. In fact, when I asked about each stanza, the most
common
response was to offer something of a paraphrase. Even when the students
recognizu, the irony within the poems, they sometimes failed to
use their
insights in their final interpretations.
Further study of different groups of readers interpreting different
types of ironic texts would be useful. However, the interviews strongly
suggest that Booth's analysis is applicable to experienced
reauers. The
readings of the students suffered because they did not recognize the
five
clues.
What can a classroom teacher do with an understhnding of huw ironic
meanings are detected and reconstructed? Researchers in the field of
metacognition in reading instruction would argue that this understanding
could be the basis of a successful instructional program.
Study II: A Comparison of the Effects of Direct. Tacit, and No Instruction
on High School Students Comprehension of Irony in Poetry
28
27
The purpose of the second study is to examine two methods of
improving students ability to interpret irony in poetry. One method of
instruction, the direct method, follows the lead of the research
on
metacognition and attempts to give students conscious control
over the
strategies that Booth identifies. The other method, the tacit method,
follows the f,i,oestions of English education theorists like Beach and
Hillocks who beli.-Ive that students will develop effective strategies for
interpreting different kinds of texts if they have extended practice with
those types of texts.
I compared the effect of the direct and tacit
treatments to each other and to no treatment groups.
Subjects
Four teachers from two suburban Chicago high schools agreed to
participate in the study. One of these teachers presented the material to
ninth graders, one presented the materials to eleventh graders, and two
presented the materials to tenth graders. In all, two hundred and
fifty-three students took both the pretest and the posttest. This number
includes seventy-two ninth graders, one hundred and nine tenth graders,
and seventy-two eleventh graders.
Procedures
I randomly assigned the experimental, control, and no instruction
treatments to the two teachers who had three similar classes. The other
two teachers had only two similar classes.
I also randomly assigned the
experimental and control treatments to these teachers. Each of these
teachers arranged for another class of the same course and level to act
as
a no treatment group.
The Treatments
29
28
The direct method of instruction thkes its inspiration from the
findings reading researchers have made on the powerful effects that
developing methcognitive understhndings of reading strategies have
on
comprehension. These researchers have considered the impact of
explicitly teaching the rules or skills
necessary to complete a reading
thsk successfully. Raphael and Pearson (1982); Raphael, Wonnecott,and
Pearson (1983); Brown, P&incsar, and Armbruster (1984); and Paris, Oka,
and De Brito (1983) are among those who have shown the
power of this
type of instruction.
Brown, Bransford, Campione, and Ferrara (1983) summarize key
findings in this area:
...
if we consider a number of instruction&
experiments that have included groups of students
differing in age or ability and that have involved
manipulation of the complexity of the skills being
taught, a gener& pattern begins to emerge. The
most basic point is that poor performance often
results in a f&lure of the learner to bring to bear
specific routines or skills importhnt for optimal
performance. In this case, readers need to be
thught explicitly what those rules are. This, in turn,
requires a dethiled theoretic& an&ysis of the
domain in question; otherwise, we cannot specify the
skills in sufficient detail to enable instruction. (pp. 140-141)
In brief, the direct instruction seeks first to build students'
awareness of the five clues that Booth identifies as the way authors
sign& that their work might be ironic by examining five cartoons by James
Thurber, each of which makes its irony clear through the use of a different
clue. In the discussion of these cartoons, the instruction also highlights
and makes explicit the three steps that readers go through in
3 0
29
reconstructing mening of Ler they have rejected the surface meaning of a
text.
With this background, students examine five to ten examples of each
clue in separate worksheets. Then students focus on the steps of
reconstruction in two worksheets. After this preparation, students begin
applying the principles they have learned to popular songs. Only after the
students should have become thoroughly familiar with the five clues and
the steps of reconstruction does the instruction proceed to actual poetry.
Students analyze the first four poems with the aid of worksheets, the
questions on which highlight the clues and aid students in reconstructing
meaning.
After students write an ironic monologue, they analyze four poems on
their own without the aid of supporting questions, relying only on their
understanding of the clues and the steps of reconstruction. This should
ensure that students monitor their understanding of the unit concepts.
After writing an essay on understanding irony, students will be given two
poems that consider man's relationship to others, only one of which is
ironic. Students will be asked to explain how they recognized the
difference between the poems.This exercise should help them develop
a
final key skill to understanding irony: knowing when to stop. That is, the
exercise should help students discriminate between what is and what is
not ironic. The direct instruction places an almost equal emphasis on
small and large group work.
The tacit method of instruction is based on the idea espoused by
Beach and Hillocks that students will develop their own strategies for
dealing with a certain type of text if they have extended practice with
31
30
that type of text. They would argue that tacit knowledge is equally
effective. Beach and Appleman (1984) explain:
From extensive reading of a certain text type,
readers acquire thcit knowledge of different text
structures. The fact that readers perceive a certain
text in terms of a certain text structure means
that they can derive meaning from that text. (p.
118)
Among English educators Hillocks has been perhaps the most
outspoken proponent of developing units of literature around works that
share similar structures.
In his The Dynamics of English Instruction
(1971, written with McCabe and McCampbell), he explains his rationale:
If a student is confronted with a series of
related literary situations that require similar
(but not the same) inferences, he will learn
what to observe and how to make the necessary
inferences. (p. 254)
To aid students in their observations, he suggests that units of
related pieces of literature be developed around a series of key questions.
Hillocks argues that repeated practice answering these focused questions
will help students develop the interpretive strategies they need to answer
them. An outstanding example of his approach is his book Satire (1974).
The two types of experimental instruction share many of the same
features. The tacit method uses all of the texts that the direct method
does. However, because the initial work in the direct instruction with the
cartoons and the worksheets takes time, the tacit method uses additional
poems. All four of the teachers of the tacit method presented five
additional ironic poems. Two teachers presented the tacit instruction
more quickly than the other teachers. These teachers also included a
culminating activity that called for students to evaluate Masters's
32
31
attitude to eight of his speakers from Spoon River Anthology. By including
these addition& poems the teachers all spent the same number of
classroom periods on the two methods of instruction.
Also, the thcit instruction does not explicitly mention irony, although
the notion is explained without the use of the term. Like the direct
instruction, the thcit instruction stresses both small and thrge group
work. The small group worksheets in the thcit instruction on poems that
have worksheets in the direct instruction are slightly modified versions of
the direct instruction worksheets that eliminate only references to the
clues and the steps of reconstruction. The worksheets on those poems
that have no worksheets in the direct instruction and the worksheets of
poems not included in the direct instruction were thken from lite.rature
texts, with the exception of the assignment on Spoon River Anthology.
with only explicit references to irony omitted. The worksheet on Spoon
River Anthology simply asked students to rank the speakers of eight poems
according to how positively Masters felt about them. Explicit references
to irony were omitted so that control teachers who were asked to explain
the term did not include any of the substance of the experiment&
instruction in their answers.
In the tacit instruction students did two similar writing assignments.
The sequence of the control instruction is slightly different to
accommodate the additional poems in a sensible format.
Each teacher spent the same number of class periods for both the direct
and thcit instruction. Teachers administered the posttest without
informing their students of the format or date of the test.
33
32
Results and Discussion
To investigate the effect that the three methods of
instruction had on
students' performance I did an analysis of variance using
the difference
scores on each of the three measures in which I am interested, using class
as the unit of analysis. These measures are the difference between
the
pretest and posttest scores on the twenty-eight item
test, the difference
between the pretest and posttest scores
on the fourteen ironic items
contained in that test, and the difference between the pretest
and postte.st
scores on eight new items. Table 3 displays summary statistics for each
of the treatments.
TABLE 3: SUMMARY STATISTICS FOR THREE MEASURES
DIRECT TREATMENT (N=66)
Difference
Difference
Score on
on 28 item
on 14 ironic
8 new items
test
items
mean
2.48
1.66
4.66
s.d.
3.43
2.36
E59
TACIT TREATMENT (N=86)
Difference
Difference
Score on
on 28 item
on 14 ironic
8 new items
test
items
mean
2.24
1.24
4.31
s.d.
2.93
2.20
E68
NO TREATMENT (N=61)
Difference
Difference
Score on
on 28 item
on 14 ironic
8 new items
test
items
mean 0.04
0.20
3.68
s.d.
2.93
2.55
1.51
3 4
33
Table 4 displays the results of the analyses
of variance. The analysis
of variance indicates that the difference
in the effect of the three
treatments is statistically significant
on all three measures. In fact, the
probability of observing differences this large by
chance is .001, or less
than one chance in a thousand. The analysis of
variance further
establishes that these differences
are a function of the difference among
tre.atments and not a function of the differences
among the four teachers
or the interaction between teachers and treatments. The analysis
of
variance does not establish which treatment
or treatments is responsible
for the stati sti cal 1 y signifi cant difference,
however.
TABLE 4: RESULTS OF ANALYSES OF VARIANCE
DIFFERENCE ON 28 ITEM TEST
SOURCE
SUM OF SQUARES OF
MEAN SQUARE
F RATIO
Teacher
5.483
3
1.828
.189
.903
Treatment
299.45 2
149.725
15.525
<.001
Teacher x
treatment
88.496
6
14.749
1.529
.169
DIFFERENCE IN 14 IRONIC ITEMS
SOURCE
SUM OF SQUARES DF
MEAN SQUARE
F RATIO
Teacher
5.844 3
1.95
.345
.793
Treatment
81.590
2
40.795
7.225
<.001
Teacher x
treatment
37.856
6
6.309
1.117
.353
SCORE ON 8 NEW ITEMS
SOURCE
SUM OF SQUARES DF
MEAN SQUARE
F RATIO
Teacher
11.378 3
3.739
1.508
.213
Treatrnent
36.703 2
18.352
7.297
<.001
Teacher x
treatment
17.660
6
2.943
1.170
.323
35
34
To specify the source of the difference, I ran
a variety of contrasts.
That is,
I analyzed the results to see if I could determine where the
differences lie. Table 5 displays those contrasts.
TABLE 5: CONTRASTS
On 28 item test
On 14 ironic items
On 6 new items
F ratio
P
F ratio
P
F ratio
Direct v No
25.054
<.001
13.569
<.001
14.494
<.001
Direct v Tacit
.120
.729
1.011
.316
2.588
.109
Tacit v No
21.925
(001
7.327
.007
5.004
.026
This table establishes that both the direct and thcit methods achieved
statistic&ly significant differences when compared to the
no treatment
group on all three measures. However, there were no statistically
significant differences between the direct and the tacit methods. Indeed,
only on the performance on the eight new items did the difference between
these two treatments approach significance (p<.109), and even here the P
value was over twice the .05 value that is usually considered es the
minimum value for establishing statistic& significance.
However, two areas appear promising for research comparing the
direct and tacit methods.
I compared the results of the direct and tacit
methods for each of the four methods. The difference between the effect
of the two methods approached significance for the ninth grade classes
with the direct method favored. This was the only group of students for
whom the difference between methods was noteworthy. This makes
sense.
It is likely that the ninth graders had the least developed interpretive
strategies. Perhaps metacognitive instruction is most effective for the
3 6
35
least experienced readers. In addition, the direct instruction appeared to
have a greater impact on the students' ability to
answer the interview
questions. For this an&ysis I compared the differences between the
students' pretreatment and posttreatment interviews
as calculated by
Rasch analysis. Three of the students who received the direct instruction
improved by more than two sthndard errors and the fourth improved by
more than one sthndard error. In contrast, although one individu& who
received the thcit instruction improved more than two standard
errors, the
other three individuals did not dernonstrate a noteworthy change. One
student who received no treatment improved one standard
error while the
other two received scores that were one standard error
worse.
Because the sample is so small it is risky making any
generalizations. However, the data are suggestive. The interview
questions were more difficult than the test questions. The data suggest
that the direct method is more useful than the tacit method in helping
students cope with more difficult thsks. This &so makes
sense.
If a task
is easy, one can do it naturally. Reflection on the
process is not
necessary. However, when a task is problematic, it may be important to
have conscious control of the strategies one may
use to accomplish it.
Studies that use tests that are thrgeted above the population's ability
would help investigate this hypothesis.
Though much addition& study is needed, the results
are promising.
The data strongly suggest that both methods of instruction
can help
students significantly improve their interpretive skills in particular
genre
in a relative1:j short period of time. Both methods were statistic&ly
superior to no treatment on each of the measur6s I considered. However,
37
36
the study does not suggest the superiority
of either the direct or tacit
method. Further research should consider the effect
of these methods in
other reading situations and with other populations
of readers.
These findings are particularly interesting
because of the little
research on effective methods of teaching literature.
They provide a
challenge to literature teachers because both
methods are substantially
different from current instructional practice,
at least as it is reflected in
literature textbooks. No text that I have
seen specifies the interpretive
strategies that skilled readers bring to
a particular task, though Hillocks
takes a step in this direction by arranging literature
units around a few
key questions. No major anthology has followed
the suggestions of Beach
or the example of Hillocks and offered units in well-defined
genre. Genre
divisions as broad as "poetry" or "the short story" give readers
little
sense of direction instead of locating them in what Booth calls
a "fairly
narrow groove."
(p. 99)
Additional Implications
One implication of my work is that we need not do research
on
teaching literature in a vacuum. While little research exists
that
evaluates methods of instruction, there are developed research
traditions
that can provide a guide. One is reading research. To this point
little has
been done to apply the findings of reading research to literature
instruction. In high schools reading is what is taught to remedial
students; literature is what is taught to the college-bound.
And never the
twain shall meet. Literature teachers seem to believe that
their business
is not the business of reading teachers. "Reading
teachers seek to build
comprehension skills," they might say. "Our students
already have them."
38
But as I have pointed out, this positini
J. Students do misread.
Reading research can help suggest methods that
may help students avoid
misreadings.
The direct method of instruction in
my study was inspired by the
work that reading researchers did on metacognition.
Future researchers
may wish to investigate other areas of reading research, for example,
questioning strategies or the use of advanced organizers.
Reading
research is a fruitful source of inspiration and guidance;
my work
suggests that researchers on teaching literature will profit if
they
consi der it.
In planning their work researchers also should consider
the body of
theory that already exists. For years, methods texts have
advocated
presenting coherent units of instruction
as an effective way to teach
literature. My work was one attempt to test this premise. Other
premises
abound. They should be tested before they
are accepted. Is small group
work more effective than large group discussions? We
pretend that it is,
but empirical support would allow teachers to
use it with more
confi dence.
In addition, researchers should look to the work of literary
critics in
planning their studies. Their theories can often be examined
empirically.
My work was inspire.d by the work of Booth, and, at least
to some extent,
supports his insights. The work of other critics has
an influence on how
teachers teach. We need to test these theories and their effects
before we
accept them.
If we are to do this, we must have instruments. My
work convinces
me that Rasch analysis is an invaluable tool for designing and validating
3 9
38
tests. As researchers develop more and
more reliable instruments, future
research will be facilitated.
In the past decade we have made great
strides in our understhnding
of how students compose and what
instruction is most effective in helping
them compose better. We need
now to direct the same energy towards
understhnding how students interpret literature
and how we can help them
do it better.
4 0
APPENDIX
41
Each of the following syen poems is followed by four statements. Please indicate whether
you
believe each statement is true (T) or false '(F). Ple.ise respond to
every statement. Circle onl
one response to each statemt:.
1.
BASE DETAILS
If I were fierce and bald and short of breath,
I'd live with scarlet Majors at the Base.
And speed gl urn heroes up the line to death.
You'd see me with my puffy, petulant face,
Guzzling and gulping in the best hotel,
Reading the Roll of Honor. "Poor young chap,"
I'd say- -"I used to know his father well;
Yes, we've lost heavily in this last scrap."
And when the war is done and youth stone dead,
I'd toddle safely home and die-
bed.
SieQiried Sassoon
The author admires officers. T
F
The author believes thot enlisted men feel honored to fight in wars.
T
F
The author believes that officers have a genuine concern for their
men.
T
F
The author believes that war is equally dangerous for officers and enlisted
men. T
F
2.
THE UNKNOWN CITIZEN
(To JS/07/M/378
This Marble Monument
Is Erected by the State)
He was found by the bureau of statistics to be
One against whom there was no official complaint,
And all reports on his conduct agree
That, in the modern sense of an old-fashioned word, he was a saint,
For in everything he did he served the Greater Community.
Except for the War till the day he retired
He worked in a factory and never got fired,
But satisfied his employers, Fudge Motors Inc.
Yet he wasn't a scab or odd in his views,
For his Union reports that he paid his dues,
(Our report on his union shows it was sound)
And our Social Psychology workers found
That he was popular with his mates and liked a drink.
The press are convinced that he bought a paper every day
And that his reactions to advertisements were normal in every
way.
42
40
Policies taken out i n his name prove that he was fully insured,
And his Health-card shows he was once in a hospital but left it cured.
Both his producers research and High-Grade living declare
He was full y sensible to the advantages of the Installment Plan
And had everything necessary to the Modern Man,
A phonograph, a radio, a car and a frigidai
re.
Our researchers Into Public Opinion are content
That he had the proper opinions for the time of
year;
When there was peace, he was for peace; when there
was war, he
went.
He was married and added five children to the population,
Which our eugenist says was the right number for a parent of his
generation,
And our teachers report that he never interfered with their education.
Was he free? Was he happy? The question is absurd:
Had anything been wrong, we would certainl y have heard.
W. 11. Auden
The author believes that the citizen's job, views, and possessions make hi
rn a unique i ndividual.
T F
The author believes that society and its institutions never complained about the citizen. T
F
The author believes the citizen was free. T
F
The author believes that the reader should approve the kind of life the citizen led. T
F
3.
"fiext to of course god america i
"next to of course god america i
land of the pilgrims' and so forth oh
say can you see by the dawn's early my
country 'tis of centuries come and go
and are no more what of it we should worry
in ever y language even deafanddumb
thy sons acclaim your glorious name by gorry
by jingo by gee by gosh by gum
why talk of beauty what could be more beau-
tiful than these happy heroic dead
who rushed like lions to the roaring slaughter
they did not stop to think they died instead
then shall the voice of liberty be mute?"
He spoke. And drank rapid] y a glass of water
e e cumrnings
43
41
The author admires the speaker's attitude towards America. T
F
The author believes the speaker is a si ncere religious
person. T
F
The author believes the happy heroic dead should have stopped to think about %AEA
they were
doing. T
F
The author believes that an understanding and appreciation of American history
are important.
T
F
4.
SONNET 130
My mistress' eyo are nothing like the sun;
Coral is far more red than her lips red;
If 31104 be white, why then her breasts are dun;
If hairs be wires, black wires grow on her head.
I have seen roo damasked, red and white,
But no such roes see I in her cheeks;
And in some perfumes i3 there more delight
Than in the breath that from my mistress reeks.
I love to hear her speak, yet well I know
That 111113iC hath a far more pleasing sound;
I grant I never saw a goddess go;
My mistress, when she walks, treads on the ground.
And yet, by heaven, I think my love as
rare
As any she belied with false compare.
William Shakespeare
The author criticizes the woman described i
n the poem because her voice is not es pleasing as
music. T
F
The author believes that the %,vornan described in the
poem is truly loved.
T
The author believes that poets should be realistic about their subjects. T
F
The author believo that the woman described in the poem has unattractive
ego. T
F
5.
SONNET 18
Shall I compare thee to a summer's day?
Thou art more lovel y and more temperate:
Rough wind do shake the darling buds of May,
And summer's lease hath all too short a date;
Some times too hot the eye of heaven shines,
And often is his gold complexion dimmed;
And every fair from fair sometimes declines,
By chance or nature's changing course untrimmed;
But thy eternal summer shall not fade,
4 4
42
Nor lose possession of the fair thou ow'st;
Nor shall death brag thou wanderst in his shade,
When in eternal li nes to time thou grow'st:
So long as men can breathe, or eyes can see,
So long lives this, and this gives life to thee.
William Shakespeare
The author believes that the woman described in the
poem is lovelier than summer. T
F
The author believes that poetry has the power to make love immortal. T
F
T he author believes that fai r thi ngs i n nature lose t hei
r fai r ness. T
F
The author believes that Me woman described in the
poem is more temperate than summer.
T
F
6.
BOOM!
SEES BOOM IN REUGION, TOO
Atlantic City, June 23, 1957 (AP). President Eisenhower's pastor said tonight
that
Americans are living in a period of "unprecedented religious activity" caused partially
by paid
vacations, the eight bour day and modern conveniences.
"These fruits of material progress," said the Rev. Edward L. R. Olsen of the National
Presbyterian Church, Washington, have provided the leisure, the
energy, and the means for a
level of human and spiritual val ues never before reached."
Here at the Vespasian-Carlton, it's just one
religious activity after another; the sky
is constantly being crossed by cruciform
airplanes, in which nobody disbelieves
for a second, and the tide, the tide
of spi ritual progress and prosperity
miraculously keeps rising, to a level
never before attai ned. The churches are full,
the beaches are full, and the filling-stations
are full, God's great ocean is full
of paid vacationers praying an eight-hour day
to the human and spiritual values, the fruits,
the leisure, the energy, and the means, Lord,
the means for the level, the unprecedented level,
and the modern conveniences, which also are full.
Never before,13 Lord, have the prayers and praises
from belfry and phonebooth, from ballpark and barbecue
the sacrifices, so endlessl y ascended.
It was not thus when Jo b 1 in Palestine
sat in the dust and cried, cried bitterl g;
when Damien2 kissed the lepers on their wounds
it was not thus; it was not thus
45
43
when Francis3 worked a fourteen-hour day
strictly for the' birds; when Dante4 took
a week's vacation without pay and it rained
part of the time, 0 Lord, it was not thus.
But now the gears mesh and the tires burn
and the ice chatters i n the shaker and the priest
in the pulpit, and Thr: Name, 0 Lord,
is kept before the public, while the fruits
ripen end religion booms end the level rises
end every modern convenience runneth over,
that it may never be with us as it hath been
with Athens and Karnak5 and Nagasaki6,
nor Thy sun for one instant refrain from 3hi ning
on the rainbow Buick by the breezeway
or the Chris Craft with the uplift life raft;
that we may continue to be the just folks we are,
plain people with ordinary superliners end
disposable diaperliners, people of the stop'n'shop
'n'pray 83 you go, of hotel, motel, boatel,
the humble pilgrims of no deposit no return
and please adjust thy clothing, who will give to Thee
if Thee will keep U3 going, our annual
Mi33 Universe, for Thy Name's Sake, Amen.
Howard Nemerov
1the
hero of an Old Testhment book who keeps his faith despite the many
happened to hi m
2,1 9th century missionary whpo cared for lepers. He eventuall
y died of
'a saint famous for his si mple life and his way with animals
48
great blian poet
5Egyptian town where the great ancient city of Thebes
once stood
6the second city destroyed by en atomic bomb
terrible things that
lepron himself.
The aut her believes that the spiritual progress of his society keeps rising. T
F
The author admires the form that the religious devotions of his society take. T
F
The author believes that the wealth of the modern world is greater than at any other time in
history. T
F
The author believes that if people are fortunate it is became God is watching out for them.
T F
4 6
44
7.
DULCE ET DECORUM EST
Bent double, like old beggars under sacks,
Knock-kneed, coughing like hags, we cursed through sludge,
Till on the haunting flares we turned our backs
And towards our distant rest began to trudge.
Men marched asleep. Many had lost their boots
But li mped on, blood shod. All went lame; all blind;
Drunk with fatigue; deaf even to the hoots
Of tired, outstripped Five- Ni nes 1 that dropped behi nd.
Gas! Gas! Quick, boys!--An ecsthsy of fumbling,
Fitti ng the cl umsy hel mets j ust i n ti me;
But someone still was yelling and stumbling
And flound'ring like a man in fire or lime...
Dim, through the misty panes and thick green light,
As under a green sea, I saw him drowning.
In all my dreams, before my helpless sight,
He plunges at me, guttering, choking, drowning.
if in some smothering dreams you too :.11d pace
Behind the wagon that we flung him
And watch the white eyes writhing if,
,
Jae,
His hangi ng face, like a devil's sick of sin;
If you could hear at every jolt, the blood
Come gargling from the froth-corrupted lungs,
Obscene as cancer, bitter as the cud
Of vile incurable sores on innocent tongues,--
My friend, you would not tell with such high zest
To children ardent for some desperate glor, y,
The old Lie: Dulce et decorum est
Pro patria mori .2
Wilfred Owen
1ges-8
hells
2It is sweet and fitting to die for one's country.
The author believes that being in a war is a horrible experience. T
F
The author believes that some people see war as a means to achieve glory. T
F
The author believes that the experience of war would change the attitude of those who glorify it.
I
F
The author believes that it is sweet and fitting to die for one's country. T
F
4 7
45
8.
DEAD BOY
The little cousin is dead, by foul subtraction,
A green bough from Virginia's aged tree,
And none of the country kin like the transaction,
Nor some of the world of outer dark, like me.
A boy not beautiful
, nor
good, nor clever,
A black cloud full of storms too hot for keeping,
A sword beneath his mother's heart-- yet never
Woman bewept her babe as this is weeping.
A pig with a pasty face, so I had said,
Squealing for cookies, kinned by poor pretense
With 3 noble house. But the little man quite de;:d,
I see the forbears antique lineaments.
The elder men have strode by the box of death
To the wide flag porch, and mutter ri ng iow send round
The bruit1 of the day.
friendly waste of breath!
Their hearts are hurt with a deep dynastic wound.
He was pale and little, the foolish neighbors sag;
The first-fruits, saith the Preacher, the Lord hath taken;
But this was the old tree's late branch wrenched
away,
Grieving the sapless limbs, the shorn and shaken.
John Crowe Ransom
1
news
The author believes that the boy was from an old, established
family. T
F
The author shares the feeling of mourning that the relatives
demonstrate. T
F
The author admires the boysfarnily. T
F
The author finds consolation in the preacher's words. T
F
9.
COMMUNITIE
Good wee must love, and must hate ill,
For ill is ill, and good good still.
But there are things indifferent,
Which wee may neither hate, nor love,
But one, and then another prove,
As wee shall find our fancy bent.
If then at first wise Nature had
4 8
46
Made women either good or bad,
Then some wee might hate, and some chuse,
But since shee did them so create,
That wee may neither love, nor hate,
Onely this rests, All, all may use.
If they were good it would be seene,
Good is as visi ble as greene,
And to all eyes it selfe betrays:
If they were bad, they could not last,
Bad dot h it selfe, and others west,
So, they deserve nor blame, nor praise.
But they are ours es fruits are ours,
He but that tests, he that devours,
And he that leaves all, doth as well:
Chang'd loves are but chang'd sorts of meat,
And when he hath the kernell este,
Who doth not fling awa y the shell?
John Donne
The author admires the reasoning the speaker uses in advancing his beliefs. T
F
The author believes that it is morall y right treat things that
are neither good nor bad in any way
we choose. T
F
The author believes that it is easy to detect goodness. T
F
The author endorses the speaker's attitude towards women. T
F
4 9
SOURCES CITED
Beach, Richard & Applernan, Deborah. (1984). Reading strategies for
expository and literary text types. In Alan C. Purves & Olive Niles
(Eds.), Becoming readers in a complex society. (pp. 115-143).
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Booth, Wayne C. (1974). A rhetoric of irony. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
Brown, Ann L., Bransford, John D., Campione, Joseph, & Ferrara, Roberta.
(1983). Learning, remembering, and understanding. In John H.
Flavell & Ellen M. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive development.
(pp. 77-176). New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Brown, Ann L., Palinscar, Annemarie, & Armbruster, Bonnie. (1984).
Instructing comprehension-fostering activities in interactive
learning situations. In Heinz Mandl, Nancy L. Stein, &
Tom Trabasso. (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text. (pp.
255-286). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaurn Associates.
Cooper, Charles. (1985). Evaluating the results of classroom literary
study. In Charles R. Cooper (Ed.), Researching response
to literature and the teaching of literature: Points of departure.
(pp. 307-331). Norwood, New Jersey: Apex Publishing Corporation.
Dietrich, Daniel J.& Behms, Richard. (1984). Annotated bibliography of
research in the teaching of literature and the teaching of writing.
Research in the Teaching of English, 18, 201-218.
Durst, Russel K. and Marshall, James D. (1984). Annotated bibliography
of
research in the teaching of English. Research in the Teaching
of
English, 18
,
417-438.
Durst, Russel K. and Marshall, James D. (1985). Annotated bibliography of
research in the teaching of English. Research in the Teaching of
English, 19 (1985): 183-204 and 405-424.
50
48
Guttman, L. (1950). The basis for scalogram analysis.
In S. Stouffer, L.
Guttman, E. Suchrnan, P. Lazarfeld, S. Star, & J. Clausen, Measurement
and prediction. (pp. 60-90) New York: Wiley.
Hillocks, George. (1974). Satire: student log. New York: Scholastic Book
Services.
Hillocks, George, McCabe, Bernard, & McCampbell, James. (1971). The
dynamics of English instruction. New York: Random House.
Kennedy, X. J. (1968). An introduction to poetry. Boston: Little, Bi-own
and Company, 1978.
Muecke, D. C. (1969). The compass of irony. Londor
& Co., Ltd.
Paris, Scott G., Oka, Evelyn R. & De Bri to, Ann Marie. (1983). Beyond
decoding: Sythesis of research on reading comprehension.
Educational Leadership, 41, 76-63.
Purves, Alan C. & Beach, Richard. (1972). Li teratur, and the reader:
Research in response to literature, reading interests, and the
teaching
of literature. Urbana: NCTE.
Raphael, T. E. & Pearson, P. D. (1982). The effect of metacognitive
awareness training on children's question-answering behavior.
(Technical Report 238). Urbana: Center for the Study of
Reading,
University of Illinois.
Raphael, T. E., Wonnecott, C. A., & Pearson, P. D. (1983) Increasing
students sensitivity to sources of information: An instructional
study in question-answer relationship. (Technical Report
264).
Urbana: Center for the Study of Reading, University of
Illinois.
Squire, James R. & Applebee, Roger K. (1966). A study of
English programs
in selected high schools which consistently educate
outstanding
students in English. Cooperative Research Project No.
1994.
University of Illinois.
51
49
Wright, Benjamin & Stone, Mark. (1979). Best test design. Chicago: MESA
Press.
52