"@80=,.D"@80=,.D
/A,9.492/@.,?4:949%@,9?4?,?4A0 4?0=,.D/A,9.492/@.,?4:949%@,9?4?,?4A0 4?0=,.D
*:7@80 >>@0 =?4.70

49,9.4,7 4?0=,.D,9/&0?4=0809?$7,9949249@>?=,74,49,9.4,7 4?0=,.D,9/&0?4=0809?$7,9949249@>?=,74,
@740&290B
(30!,>:9'.3::7:1@>490>>(30:77020:1+4774,8,9/!,=D
@740290B8,>:9B80/@
,E07,?08,9
@>?=,74,9'.3::7:1@>490>>)94A0=>4?D:1"0B':@?3+,70>
3-,?08,9@9>B0/@,@
'@>,9(3:=;
)('@>490>>'.3::7)94A0=>4?D:1(0.39:7:2D'D/90D
>@>,9?3:=;@?>0/@,@
:77:B?34>,9/,//4?4:9,7B:=6>,?3??;>/424?,7.:88:9>@>10/@9@80=,.D
$,=?:1?3049,9.0,9/49,9.4,7!,9,20809?:88:9>
&0.:8809/0/4?,?4:9&0.:8809/0/4?,?4:9
290B@740&,E07,?08,9,9/'@>,9(3:=;49,9.4,7 4?0=,.D,9/&0?4=0809?$7,9949249
@>?=,74,
"@80=,.D
>>=?4.70#3??;/C/:4:=2
@?3:=>=0?,49.:;D=423?:1?304=8,?0=4,7@9/0=,=0,?4A0:88:9>":9:880=.4,7??=4-@?4:9 4.09>0
49,9.4,7 4?0=,.D,9/&0?4=0809?$7,9949249@>?=,74,49,9.4,7 4?0=,.D,9/&0?4=0809?$7,9949249@>?=,74,
->?=,.?->?=,.?
49,9.4,774?0=,.D,9/9@80=,.D,=0.7:>07D?40/@=?30=8:=0G9,9.4,774?0=,.D3,>-009>3:B9?:=07,?0
?:48;:=?,9?G9,9.4,7-03,A4:=>(34>>?@/D0C,8490>?30=07,?4:9>34;-0?B009G9,9.4,774?0=,.D,9/
=0?4=0809?;7,99492@>492,80,>@=0?3,?49.7@/0><@0>?4:9>=0<@4=4929@80=,.D+048;70809?,
.@>?:84E0/>@=A0D?:,=0;=0>09?,?4A0>,8;70:1@>?=,74,9>#A0=,77B0G9/,22=02,?070A07>:1
G9,9.4,774?0=,.D>4847,=?:.:8;,=,-70.:@9?=40>B4?3?30D:@9270,>?0/@.,?0/?3:>09:?08;7:D0/,9/
?3:>09:?49?307,-:=1:=.08:>?,?=4>6#@=G9,9.4,774?0=,.D80,>@=04>;:>4?4A07D=07,?0/?:=0?4=0809?
;7,9949249:@=>,8;70
0DB:=/>0DB:=/>
G9,9.4,774?0=,.D=0?4=0809?;7,99492
=0,?4A0:88:9> 4.09>0=0,?4A0:88:9> 4.09>0
(34>B:=64>74.09>0/@9/0=,=0,?4A0:88:9>??=4-@?4:9":9.:880=.4,7 4.09>0
:A0=$,20::?9:?0:A0=$,20::?9:?0
@740290B3:7/>?30:39",7?:9(0=83,4=,9/4>,9>>:.4,?0$=:10>>:=:149,9.0,9/
.:9:84.>,??30!,>:9'.3::7:1@>490>>,??30:77020:1+4774,8,9/!,=D'30.:8;70?0/30=$3
4949,9.0,?:>?:9:7702049B34.34>?30>,80D0,=>305:490/?30+4774,8,9/!,=D1,.@7?D'30
4>,7>:,&0>0,=.3>>:.4,?0B4?3:>?:9:77020F>09?0=1:=&0?4=0809?&0>0,=.3,9/,(&
9>?4?@?0077:B
,E07,?08,94>,9>>:.4,?0$=:10>>:=49$09>4:9.:9:84.>49?30'.3::7:1&4>6,9/.?@,=4,7
'?@/40>,9/4=0.?:=:1?3009?=01:=$09>4:9>,9/'@;0=,99@,?4:9,??30)94A0=>4?D:1"0B':@?3
+,70>,9/,9>>:.4,?09A0>?42,?:=B4?3?30@>?=,74,9&0>0,=.3:@9.4709?=01:=C.07709.049
$:;@7,?4:92492&0>0,=.3$&'30.:8;70?0/,$349.:9:84.>,??30)94A0=>4?D:1"0B':@?3
+,70>$=4:=?:5:49492?30)94A0=>4?D:1"0B':@?3+,70>,E07B:=60/49?30(,C,?4:9$:74.D4A4>4:9:1
?30@>?=,74,9(=0,>@=D
'@>,9(3:=;4>$=:10>>:=:149,9.0,9/'@;0=,99@,?4:949?3049,9.0=:@;,??30)94A0=>4?D:1
(0.39:7:2D'D/90D)(',9/,808-0=:1?3009?=01:=?30'?@/D:13:4.0'30.:8;70?0/30=$349
.:9:84.>,??30)94A0=>4?D:1"0B':@?3+,70>49,9/3,>-009,808-0=:1?30,.@7?D,??30
)('@>490>>'.3::7>49.0?309
(34>?3080.:770.?4:9G9,9.4,774?0=,.D4>,A,47,-7049"@80=,.D3??;>/424?,7.:88:9>@>10/@9@80=,.DA:7
4>>,=?
Introduction
Financial literacy scores relate positively to numeracy skills. It is difficult for
individuals to make effective financial decisions if they are unable to apply basic
mathematical calculations to their situation. Decisions such as allocating assets in
retirement portfolios, determining saving rates, evaluating mortgage product
options, and/or managing credit card debt require individuals to possess an
understanding of how to calculate compound interest as well as an asset’s risk and
return. Individuals must also be capable of interpreting the results. Without this
ability, the decision maker cannot adequately consider his or her options. The link
between quantitative literacy and financial literacy is supported by research.
Gilliland et al. (2011) highlight the positive association in their recent analysis of
Michigan State University students. Furthermore, a close examination of one of
the most popular financial literacy measuresthree survey questions that have
been dubbed the Big Threereveals that mathematical calculations are necessary
to correctly answer two of the three questions. As a result, studies of the effect of
financial literacy on behavior should be of particular interest to researchers in the
field of numeracy.
This paper uses the aforementioned Big Three measure to learn more about
financial literacy and how it relates to retirement planning in Australia. As
changes in retirement systems around the world produce shifts toward more
personal responsibility for financial decision-making, research measuring
financial knowledge and assessing how it relates to financial decisions is
becoming increasingly important. Our paper extends research recently conducted
for a global project on financial literacy and contributes to the literature by
providing new results for Australia that are directly comparable to other country
studies. Notably, before this global initiative, it was often difficult to compare
financial literacy studies due to the various methods for measuring financial
literacy and for analyzing the resulting data. This paper also adds to the literature
by examining a country whose retirement system features many personal choice
elements, making it a compelling and relevant case to study in this context.
The Australian experience is important to study because Australia was one of
the first developed countries to introduce mandatory private saving as the main
earnings-related component of the country’s retirement income system.
Consequently, almost all adult Australians are required to interact with
increasingly complex private and public arrangements for retirement
accumulation and decumulation and are exposed to investment, inflation, and
1
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
longevity risks.
1
The mandatory private saving program is referred to as the
Superannuation Guarantee in Australia. This program requires employers to make
mandatory contributions to a retirement plan of the employees choice.
2
Currently, about 95% of full-time workers are covered by this program (ABS
2012). Generally, these plans are designed as defined contribution plans very
similar to US 401(k) plans. The mandatory contributions by employers are
substantial and are currently increasing from 9% of the employee’s salary to 12%.
When the system is fully mature, the balances in these plans will be the key
component of accumulated retirement savings. Australians may also make
voluntary contributions to these plans and contribute to other personal savings
accounts to prepare for retirement. Finally, Australians who meet certain income
and asset criteria are also eligible for retirement support from a government
program called the Age Pension, which is financed through general tax revenues.
The Age Pension provides basic financial support, and in most cases does not
replace pre-retirement standards of living.
While Australian retirement savers do not have to decide whether to
participate in retirement saving plans, as they are mandatory, they are responsible
for decisions relating to the plan in which superannuation savings are managed
and accumulate (including whether to create their own self-managed
superannuation fund” [SMSF] plan), for plan management (such as consolidation
of multiple plans), for choice of investments (from increasingly long menus of
single- and multi-manager diversified and single options, and often individual
asset classes), for whether to make or increase voluntary contributions (for which
the tax rules differ by type and contribution amount), for whether to seek and use
financial advice, and for which benefit(s) to take at retirement. These decisions
can be overwhelming. While financial advice is readily available, it is not clear
whether ordinary Australians can afford advisor fees and have the skills and
experience to discern advice quality (ASIC 2012). Regardless, the efficient
functioning of the system depends on participants being well-informed and having
sufficient financial skills. This paper provides additional insight into whether this
is a valid presumption.
All of the above suggest that Australian workers and retirees face
considerable challenges navigating the complex financial products and policies
required for retirement planning. Previous literature has identified poor financial
literacy and lack of knowledge about superannuation across the Australian
population but has not specifically linked objective measures of financial literacy
1
A short description of the system can be found in Agnew (2013). A more detailed description can
be found in Bateman et al. (2013).
2
These plans are called superannuation funds in Australia. Given that the word fund may cause
confusion for non-Australians who often think of a fund as a single investment option, we will
refer to superannuation funds as plans in this paper.
2
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
with retirement planning (ANZ 2011). Croy et al. (2010) investigate how self-
assessed (rather than objective) financial knowledge relates to two financial
behaviors, specifically the intention to contribute extra to retirement plans and the
intention to change investment allocations. Bateman et al. (2012) measure
financial literacy consistent with our proposed approach but do not relate financial
literacy to financial behaviors, such as retirement planning.
3
In this paper, we use a new customized survey administered to a
representative sample of 1,024 Australians over age 18 from the Pureprofile Web
Panel of over 600,000 Australians to examine the relationship between financial
literacy and retirement planning. Overall we find that aggregate levels of financial
literacy in Australia are similar to comparable countries with the young, women,
those who are least educated, those who are not employed, and those not in the
labor force most likely to exhibit low financial literacy levels. Our age results are
somewhat difficult to interpret given a potential selection bias. We discuss
possible explanations for this in the text.
This paper is laid out as follows: In the next section we describe the
important features of our data set and present summary statistics related to
financial literacy. Following that, we examine how financial literacy relates to
retirement planning. Our final section concludes.
Data Overview and Summary Statistics
To study the relationship between financial literacy and retirement planning, we
commissioned a new survey of the Australian population. The survey used the
Pureprofile Web Panel and was fielded in June 2012 via the Internet. The
Pureprofile Panel includes over 600,000 Australians. Our final sample of 1,024
individuals was designed to be representative of Australia’s general adult
population. Survey respondents were required to be over 18. Pureprofile
compensated individuals completing the survey for their participation.
Respondents were not required to be the head of the household or the person
responsible for making financial decisions.
In terms of response rates, a traditional response rate measure could not be
computed because online surveys are administered in a different manner than
standard telephone and paper surveys. Therefore, we report the completion rate, a
commonly used metric for measuring responses to online surveys. For this survey,
Pureprofile sent survey invitations to individuals in their established pool who
met the study criteria. Out of the 1,245 who entered the survey, 1,024 (82.2%)
3
Gerrans et al. (2009) and ASIC (2011) provide useful summaries of research related to
Australian financial literacy undertaken in the past few years.
3
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
completed all the questions. A small number (6.1%) were screened out due to
non-consent or because the quota for the demographic they represented had been
filled. The remaining 11.7% started the survey but did not complete it.
While the focus of this paper is on retirement planning and basic financial
literacy responses, the survey also included questions to test the respondents
knowledge of Australia’s superannuation system. In addition, measures of
personality traits, numeracy skills, financial behavior, attitude toward and use of
financial planners, and perceptions of time until retirement were included. These
factors will be studied in future papers.
Findings Regarding Financial Literacy
In order to evaluate the financial literacy of Australians, we asked survey
participants three financial literacy questions that address basic economics and
finance concepts. The responses to these questions provide financial literacy
measures that are comparable with results from other papers.
4
The three
questions, called the Big Three, were developed by Lusardi and Mitchell (2011a)
and have been frequently used in other literature, including a series of papers
published in a special issue of the Journal of Pension Economics and Finance that
focused on financial literacy and retirement planning in eight countries including
Germany, the Netherlands, Sweden, Japan, Italy, the United States, Russia, and
New Zealand.
5
The wording of the questions is as follows (correct answers are indicated with
two asterisks):
Understanding of Interest Rate (Numeracy): Suppose you had $100
in a savings account and the interest rate was 2% per year. After 5
years, how much do you think you would have in the account if you
left the money to grow?
More than $102 **
Exactly $102
Less than $102
Do not know
Refuse to answer
Understanding of Inflation. Imagine that the interest rate on your
savings account was 1% per year and inflation was 2% per year. After
4
These basic financial literacy questions have been asked in other surveys conducted by the
authors using Pureprofile’s Web Panel. However, there is a low probability that individuals in this
sample have seen the questions before in one of these surveys.
5
See Alessie et al. 2011; Alemenberg and Säve-Söderbergh 2011; Bucher-Koenen and Lusardi
2011; Crossan et al. 2011; Fornero and Monticone 2011; Klapper and Panos 2011; Lusardi and
Mitchell 2011b; and Sekita 2011.
4
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
1 year, how much would you be able to buy with the money in this
account?
More than today
Exactly the same
Less than today **
Do not know
Refuse to answer
Understanding of Risk Diversification. Buying shares in a single
company usually provides a safer return than buying units in a
managed share fund.
6
True
False **
Do not know
Refuse to answer
The first two questions address economic topics related to saving for
retirement, including calculating interest rates and understanding the effect of
inflation on purchasing power. Correct responses to these questions require that
one has numeracy skills. The third question is related to investments and is
designed to measure understanding of the concept of diversification.
Table 1 provides a summary of respondents’ answers. Two stars denote again
the correct answers for each question. The sample is broken down into two
groups: the full sample, which includes retired and non-retired individuals age 18
to 85, and the working-adults sample, which also includes non-retired and retired
individuals age 25 to 65. The latter sample will be the main focus of the paper.
Demographics for the working-adults sample group compared to the national
population in this age group can be found in the appendix. Our working-adults
group is younger and slightly more educated than the national sample. Our
analysis uses unweighted data. Due to possible selectivity issues associated with
the participation of older people in online surveys, in Appendix Table A2, we
specifically compare the demographics for the over-65 sample with the population
of that same age. Here we find that our older sample is slightly younger and more
educated than the general population of the same age. Furthermore, our sample
has a greater proportion of males than the general population.
Overall, there is little difference between the working-adults sample and the
full sample. In both samples, more respondents answered the interest rate question
correctly compared to any other question. In fact, roughly 83% of the respondents
6
This question was slightly reworded for the Australian context from the original. The original
sentence read “Buying a single company’s stock usually provides a safer return than a stock
mutual fund.”
5
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Table 1
Summary Statistics on Three Financial Literacy Questions (%)
Full Sample (%)
Age 2565 (%)
(A) Interest Question
More than $102**
83.11%
83.09%
Exactly $102
4.10%
4.11%
Less than $102
5.57%
5.68%
DK
6.45%
6.28%
RF
0.78%
0.85%
(B) Inflation Question
More than today
9.77%
8.94%
Exactly the same
7.23%
7.13%
Less than today**
69.34%
70.77%
DK
12.99%
12.44%
RF
0.68%
0.72%
(C) Risk Question
Correct (false)**
54.69%
55.07%
Incorrect (true)
6.74%
6.28%
DK
37.60%
37.44%
Refuse
0.98%
1.21%
(D) Cross-question
consistency
Interest and Inflation
62.89%
63.65%
All correct
42.68%
42.87%
None correct
8.59%
8.09%
At least 1 DK
41.31%
41.06%
All DK
4.49%
4.35%
Number of Observations
1,024
828
Notes: DK indicates respondent replied with “do not know”; RF indicates
respondent refused to answer the question.
correctly recognized that their money would grow due to interest earnings to more
than $102. In terms of international comparison, rates of correct responses here
were similar to the Netherlands and Germany, and 1015 percentage points above
Japan and the United States.
Respondents’ accuracy fell with the inflation
question to rates lower than those elicited from surveys in the Netherlands and
Germany. About 69% of Australian respondents answered this question correctly
and almost 13% responded that they did not know the answer. The most
challenging question for Australians to answer was the risk diversification
question. Over one-third of respondents indicated that they did not know the
answer to this question, and only slightly over half were able to give correct
answers. The rate of correct responses was comparable to the United States, the
Netherlands, and Italy, but well below Sweden and Germany. While the rate of
do not knowresponses was higher for Australia than for most countries (apart
from Japan), the similar rate of correct responses suggests that the Australian
sample disproportionately chose do not knowcompared with respondents from
other countries.
Considering the questions together, a positive correlation between the correct
responses to each question was found but these correlations were never greater
6
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
than 0.35. The positive but low correlations are consistent with Lusardi and
Mitchells (2011b) findings. As suggested in their paper, the low correlations may
indicate that the three questions address different areas of financial literacy. In
total, only 63% of both samples correctly answered the interest and inflation
questions. This percentage falls to approximately 43% when responses to the risk
diversification question are incorporated. This response rate is similar to that of
the Netherlands (Alessie et al. 2011) and about ten percentage points higher than
that of U.S. respondents (Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b). Even more important,
nearly half of the respondents (approximately 41%) answered do not knowto at
least one question. This is notable because Lusardi and Mitchell (2011a) find that
those individuals who tend to respond with “do not knowoften know the least.
Who Is Financially Illiterate?
Table 2 breaks down the responses to the financial literacy questions by
sociodemographic characteristics. The table reports responses to individual
questions and overall responses. Obvious patterns emerge.
Table 2
Distribution of Responses to Financial Literacy Questions by Age, Sex, Education,
and Employment Status (%)
Interest
Inflation
Risk
Overall 3 measures
n
Correct
DK
Correct
DK
Correct
DK
3 correct
>=1 DK
79%
8%
55%
19%
42%
50%
31%
54%
359
85%
6%
69%
13%
58%
35%
44%
39%
324
86%
5%
86%
6%
62%
29%
52%
32%
239
86%
5%
82%
9%
71%
22%
58%
26%
102
85%
7%
74%
11%
62%
29%
52%
31%
490
82%
6%
65%
14%
48%
46%
34%
51%
534
80%
9%
63%
19%
53%
41%
38%
47%
283
81%
8%
66%
16%
43%
49%
34%
53%
238
86%
7%
71%
14%
55%
38%
45%
39%
122
87%
2%
76%
6%
65%
27%
52%
29%
238
84%
5%
73%
8%
60%
30%
50%
33%
143
82%
6%
80%
11%
56%
35%
48%
38%
84
70%
15%
51%
30%
38%
51%
28%
57%
74
85%
4%
63%
16%
43%
49%
29%
54%
156
83%
6%
70%
11%
56%
37%
44%
40%
648
88%
6%
81%
10%
70%
21%
57%
25%
146
Total Sample Size: 1,024
Note: The not in labor force category includes individuals who indicate that they are not in the labor force because they
are a caregiver, a student, or for some other reason. The working category includes part-time and full-time workers. It
also includes self-
employed workers. Therefore, the sum of all the employment categories will add to greater than the
sample size as 84 self-employed workers are double counted. Data are unweighted and DK
indicates respondent
answered “do not know.
7
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
For each question, younger individuals tended to respond less accurately than
their older counterparts. This is consistent with findings in other countries
(Lusardi and Mitchell 2011c). The pattern is most evident in the last column,
where the percentage of the sample answering all the questions correctly is
reported. Only 31% of individuals under 35 answered all the questions correctly
compared to 58% of those older than 65. This pattern reverses itself when
examining the do not knowresponses. Over half of the respondents under 35
answered at least one question with do not knowcompared to only 26% of the
oldest group. The differences between age groups are largest for the inflation and
risk questions.
7
Women also answered relatively fewer questions correctly than men. While
for the interest rate question the responses are fairly consistent between the sexes,
differences are more marked for the inflation and risk questions. In addition, and
mirroring other studies, women were more likely to respond with “do not know.
In the full sample, over half of the women responded do not knowto at least
one question, while only 31% of males did so.
We find that education is positively related to financial literacy. In Table 2,
we break our sample into several categories. The first category includes
individuals with a high school education or less. The next two categories include
individuals who have received education from a Technical and Further Education
(TAFE) institute or a similar school. TAFE institutes provide tertiary-level
vocational education and training in Australia and can also offer matriculation
courses for students who did not complete high school. Students can receive
certificates, as well as diplomas or advanced diplomas from these schools. The
next category includes individuals with bachelor’s degrees from a university or
equivalent school. The final category includes masters and doctorate degrees.
Comparing across the three questions, the high school or less and TAFE
(Certificate) groups replied more often with do not know.The TAFE (Diploma)
group tended to perform just as well as the bachelor’s and graduate degree groups
when answering the first two questions, which happen to be the questions
requiring numeracy skills. These results naturally follow from the fact that
graduates earning TAFE certificates and diplomas encompass a wide range of
experiences and abilities, from self-employed, skilled tradespeople and designers
to individuals who did not graduate from high school and received only very basic
training for low-skill employment. Within this group, the prerequisite skills,
which can include numeracy, required to earn a TAFE diploma or advanced
diploma are far greater than those required for a certificate and this appears to
translate into the scores. As with the previous finding, respondents tended to
7
Respondents over age 65 are less representative of the general population than respondents of
working age. Males, younger respondents, and the better educated are oversampled in this group.
See Appendix Table A2.
8
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
answer the interest question most correctly followed by the inflation question.
Interestingly, we find the non-numeracyrelated third question provides the most
distinction between university degree holders and others. For this question, the
TAFE diploma holders perform like individuals with a high school education or
less and the TAFE certificate holders perform the worst. Across the board,
individuals in the university categories perform better overall and answer least
often with do not know.”
Finally, we find that categorizing people by employment status highlights
groups with lower financial literacy. For example, respondents who demonstrate
the most difficulty answering the questions are those who are either not employed
and actively seeking work or those who are not in the labor force because they are
caregivers, students, or cannot participate for some other reason. We found that
only 28% (29%) of the not employed (those not in the labor force) group could
answer all of the questions correctly compared to 44% of workers, 48% of self-
employed workers, and 57% of the retired group.
In summary, financial illiteracy is more prevalent among certain demographic
groups. These groups are younger individuals, women, those with less education,
and those who are not employed or not in the labor force.
Planning for Retirement
In this section, we investigate whether financial literacy relates to retirement
planning in Australia. Prior research suggests that different measures of financial
sophistication and literacy relate to important investment behaviors. For example,
Calvet et al. (2009) find a relationship between demographic variables related to
financial sophistication and investment mistakes. Other papers suggest
connections between financial literacy and stock market participation, borrowing,
and mutual fund selection (Lusardi and Tufano 2009; Christelis et al. 2010; van
Rooij et al. 2011; Hastings and Mitchell 2011). Finally, a growing body of
research finds that financial literacy relates to retirement planning, which may
lead to greater wealth (for example, Ameriks et al. 2003; Behrman et al. 2010;
Lusardi 2009; Lusardi and Mitchell 2011a). It is these papers and recent findings
from other countries that provide the motivation for the following analysis.
In order to assess how financial literacy relates to retirement planning, we
asked participants the following question about their retirement planning efforts:
Have you ever tried to work out how much you need to save for
retirement?
This question has been slightly modified for the Australian context from the
retirement planning question posed in the U.S. Health and Retirement Survey
(HRS) and used in Lusardi and Mitchells papers (2011a, 2011b). The question
9
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
requires a simple yes or no reply. For this analysis, we restrict our sample to
individuals who indicated that they are not retired and are age 2565. This was
necessary given the focus on retirement planning and to allow comparability with
other studies.
We found that only 32% of the non-retired sample of 764 individuals have
attempted to work out how much they need to save. The patterns found within
each sociodemographic group seem to mirror the relationships observed with
financially literacy. While significance is not tested, males plan more than
females and individuals not working by choice or who are seeking jobs appear to
plan less. In terms of planning and age, a notable increase in planners is evident in
the 50- to 65-year-old age group (48%) relative to those under 50. For those under
50, the percentage who are planners ranges from 27% to 29%, depending on the
age category.
To determine whether planning relates positively to financial literacy, we
divided the nonretired sample into two groups: planners and nonplanners. Table 3
reports the percentage of planners and nonplanners who answered each financial
literacy question correctly. Planners were more successful at answering each
question compared to nonplanners. The largest difference we found relates to the
risk diversification question. For this question, 67% of the planners chose the
right answer versus 47% of the nonplanners. Similar differences are found once
all the literacy questions are combined. We found that just over half (55%) of the
Table 3
Financial Literacy of Planners and Non-Planners
Planners
Non-
Planners
Interest Rate Question
Correct
88%
80%
DK
1%
9%
Inflation Question
Correct
77%
67%
DK
5%
16%
Risk Diversification Question
Correct
67%
47%
DK
23%
47%
Summary
Correct: Interest and Inflation
71%
59%
Correct: all three
55%
35%
>=1 DK
25%
51%
Average number of questions correct
2.32
1.94
Average number of DKs
0.29
0.72
Sample Size
250
514
planners answered all three questions correctly versus only 35% of the
nonplanners. Furthermore, nonplanners seemed to be less confident or at least
more willing to reveal their lack of knowledge by responding do not know.
10
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
Approximately half of the nonplanners answered at least one question with “do
not knowcompared to only a quarter (25%) of the planners.
A Multivariate Model of Planning and Financial Literacy
In this section, we examine the relationship between financial literacy and
retirement planning using a multivariate regression framework. Using an indicator
variable for retirement planning as the dependent variable, first we estimate an
Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) model. The dependent variable equals one if
respondents answered affirmatively to our retirement planning question and zero
otherwise. Consistent with prior literature, we include numerous control variables
including indicator variables for homeownership, self-employment, and not
employed. We also control for each respondent’s household income. We include
age and age-squared to allow flexibility in the relationship between age and
retirement planning. Possible liquidity constraints and household income shocks
are captured by two variables: a dummy variable
8
that equals one if the
respondent or someone in the respondent’s family has ever experienced a drastic
and unexpected fall in savings or income and a variable representing the number
of children in the household. We define all of our variables in Appendix Table
A3, Panels A and B.
In Table 4 we report the results from four specifications using different
financial literacy measures. The financial literacy measure all three correctin
the first specification is an indicator variable that equals one if the respondent
answered all the financial literacy questions correctly and zero if not. The
measure in the second specification, total number correct,” equals the number of
questions answered correctly. The third specification includes separate indicator
variables for each financial literacy question. The variable equals one if the
specific question is answered correctly. The final measure in the last specification
is the sum of the do not knowresponses of each respondent.
The regression results show there is a statistically significant relationship
between financial literacy and retirement planning which holds for each measure
of financial literacy. The first specification suggests that the probability of being a
planner increases by 12.3 percentage points if individuals can answer all three
financial literacy questions correctly. In the second specification, each question
answered correctly raises the chances of planning by nearly 6 percentage points.
In the third specification, only the risk question out of the three financial literacy
8
In empirical economics, binary (or indicator or categorical) variables are typically referred to as
dummy variables. These variables takes the value of zero or one to indicate the absence or
presence of some categorical effect.
11
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Table 4
OLS Estimates of Retirement Planning on Financial Literacy
Dependent Variable=1 if
planner (0 else)
(1)
(2)
(3)
(4)
All three correct
0.123***
(0.04)
Total number correct
0.059***
(0.02)
Inflation correct
0.054
(0.04)
Interest correct
-0.022
(0.04)
Risk correct
0.135***
(0.04)
Total number DKs
-0.113***
(0.02)
Age
-0.052***
-0.053***
-0.055***
-0.056***
(0.01)
(0.01)
(0.01)
(0.01)
Age squared
0.001***
0.001***
0.001***
0.001***
(0.00)
(0.00)
(0.00)
(0.00)
Female
0.008
0.001
0.004
0.000
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.03)
Education, TAFE (Certificate)
0.065
0.065
0.070
0.068
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
Education, TAFE (Diploma)
0.096
0.096
0.097
0.095
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
Education, bachelor’s degree
0.143***
0.142***
0.143***
0.119**
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
Education, graduate degree
0.134**
0.138**
0.138**
0.122**
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
Single
-0.039
-0.038
-0.039
-0.031
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
Divorced or separated
0.086
0.091
0.090
0.093
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
Widow
0.332**
0.336**
0.347**
0.295**
(0.14)
(0.14)
(0.14)
(0.13)
Number of children
-0.005
-0.004
-0.004
-0.000
(0.02)
(0.02)
(0.02)
(0.02)
Income $20,800$41,599
-0.072
-0.063
-0.065
-0.053
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
Income $41,600$67,599
0.024
0.026
0.021
0.018
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
Income greater than $67,600
0.053
0.060
0.050
0.057
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
(0.05)
Homeowner
0.083**
0.085**
0.083**
0.087**
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
(0.04)
Self-employed
0.016
0.016
0.022
0.019
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
(0.06)
Not working
0.026
0.039
0.034
0.048
(0.07)
(0.07)
(0.07)
(0.07)
Had income shock
0.015
0.015
0.016
0.010
(0.03)
(0.03)
(0.03)
(0.03)
Constant
1.073***
1.027***
1.081***
1.286***
(0.30)
(0.30)
(0.30)
(0.30)
Observations
751
751
751
751
R-squared
0.122
0.119
0.128
0.139
Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses. *** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1.
12
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
This is consistent with Lusardi and Mitchell’s (2011b) findings for the United
States. Responding withdo not knowalso has significant explanatory power. In
fact, the chances of being a planner decrease by 11.3 percentage points for each
do not knowresponse. As a robustness check, we repeated the analysis with a
probit estimation. The relationships between the planning variable and the
different financial literacy variables remained qualitatively the same.
The regression results also suggest that certain demographic factors relate to
planning. For example, we find a nonlinear relationship between age and
planning, captured by the age-squared variable. The likelihood of planning,
measured by the marginal effect of age, increases near midlife. The marginal
effect of an additional year is positive from around age 40, suggesting a
gradually increasing interest in planning starting in midlife.
9
Earlier surveys show
that retirement planning is sporadic at best among Australian pre-retirees (Agnew
et al. 2013).
Education also relates to planning. Comparing individuals with a high school
degree or lower education to those earning a university degree, either a bachelor’s
degree or a graduate degree, increases the probability of planning between 12 and
14 percentage points. Homeownership, which is concentrated in the upper two-
thirds of the wealth distribution, also increases planning (by 8–9 percentage
points). As well as being wealthier, home-owning households have managed a
long-term financial contract with a mortgage provider and are likely to have built
up some financial competence that spills over into retirement planning. Overall,
the largest effect on retirement planning is widowhood status. Individuals who are
widowed are more likely to plan for retirement. This may be because, following
the death of a spouse, individuals are forced to carefully consider their finances if
they have not already done so. Interestingly, income was not statistically
significant, but does play a large role in the findings from other countries (for
example, Lusardi and Mitchell, 2011b).
In total, the results presented are similar to findings from other countries.
However, like the other studies, direction of causality is uncertain, whether from
financial literacy to retirement planning or the other way around. As noted by
others, individuals may become more financially literate because they plan, and/or
planning and financial knowledge may be driven by underlying characteristics.
This is an endogeneity problem that requires more sophisticated estimation
techniques. Other considerations include the possibility of measurement errors
(Lusardi and Mitchell 2011b, van Rooij et al. 2011). Since endogeneity problems
and measurement error can affect estimated coefficients in different directions, we
cannot forecast the sign of possible biases with any certainty.
9
We calculate this age turning point using coefficients estimated to eight significant figures. Due
to sensitivity to rounding, readers will not be able to replicate this age finding using the estimates
reported in Table 4 because they are rounded to three significant figures.
13
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
To address these issues, we use an instrumental variables (IV) approach. The
challenge of using IV estimation is finding valid instruments that are correlated
with financial literacy measures but uncorrelated with the error term. Motivated
by Alessie et al. (2011), we constructed instruments based on the financial
experiences of respondents’ siblings and parents. Specifically, we asked if
respondents had siblings and then inquired whether their oldest sibling was in a
worse, better, or similar financial situation. From these responses, we created
indicator variables for siblings in worse and better financial situations. Alessie et
al. (2011) propose these variables as suitable instruments for financial literacy
because, while individuals cannot control a sibling’s financial situation, they can
learn from their siblings’ financial experiences. What they learn likely improves
their financial literacy but is not generated by their own planning behavior. In
addition, we include an indicator variable that equals one if the respondents
thought their parents had a good or intermediate understanding of financial
matters and another indicator variable that equals one if they have ever received
workplace financial education.
10
These instruments have been used in IV
modeling of financial literacy and retirement planning in other developed
countries (Alessie et al. 2011; Lusardi and Mitchell 2010).
Table 5 reports the results from the first and second stage regressions using
Generalized Method of Moments (GMM) estimation that allows for computation
of robust standard errors. We report only the estimates for one specification of the
financial literacy variable (all questions are answered correctly) because the
proposed instrumental variables were strongest in this case. As noted above, two
statistical properties are needed to ensure that our selected instruments serve their
purpose. The first is that the instrument is correlated with the financial literacy
variable of interest, and the second is that it is uncorrelated with the random error
in the model. Table 5, Panel B, tests for the predictive power of the instruments
10
We followed the exact methodology for constructing the instrumental variables used in Alessie
et al. (2011). Regarding siblings, we asked, “Would you say that your oldest [brother/sister] is in
worse than, better than, or about the same financial situation as you? To measure parents
financial understanding we asked, “How would you assess your parents understanding of
financial matters? Think about the parent that is or was mostly responsible for the major financial
decisions. Respondents ranked the parent’s knowledge using a 7-point scale (1 for very low; 7 for
very high). Consistent with Alessie et al. (2011), we created an indicator variable equal to one if
the parent was judged to have intermediate or high knowledge measured by a response of 4 or
greater. We also included an indicator equal to one if respondents did not answer or answered “do
not know. Finally, following Lusardi and Mitchell (2010) we asked about workplace education:
Did any of the firms you have worked for (including your current employer) offer financial
education programs such as retirement seminars?” An indicator variable was coded one if the
respondents answered yes.
14
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
Table 5
Panel A: IV Estimates of Financial Literacy Impact on Retirement
Planning
Dependent Variable Equals
First Stage
Second Stage
All three correct
OLS
1 if planner (0 else)
GMM
All three correct
0.861***
(0.27)
Age
0.011
-0.057***
(0.01)
(0.02)
Age squared
0.000
0.001***
(0.00)
(0.00)
Female
-0.107***
0.094*
(0.04)
(0.05)
Education, TAFE (Certificate)
-0.006
0.065
(0.05)
(0.06)
Education, TAFE (Diploma)
0.041
0.053
(0.06)
(0.08)
Education, Bachelor Degree
0.140***
0.034
(0.05)
(0.08)
Education, Graduate Degree
0.075
0.069
(0.06)
(0.08)
Widow
-0.197
0.465**
(0.12)
(0.19)
Single
0.003
-0.037
(0.05)
(0.05)
Divorced or separated
-0.007
0.076
(0.06)
(0.08)
Number of children
-0.023
0.009
(0.02)
(0.02)
Income, $20,800-$41,599
-0.035
-0.056
(0.05)
(0.06)
Income, $41,600-$67,599
0.022
-0.007
(0.05)
(0.07)
Income, Greater than $67,600
0.133
-0.074
(0.05)
(0.08)
Homeowner
0.072*
0.025
(0.04)
(0.06)
Self-employed
0.026
0.035
(0.06)
(0.08)
Not working
0.001
0.038
(0.07)
(0.09)
Had income shock
0.068*
-0.051
(0.04)
(0.05)
Sibling in Worse Situation
0.053
(0.04)
Sibling in Better Situation
0.039
(0.05)
Parents are good financial
0.062
decision makers
(0.04)
Don't know if parents are good
-0.102
financial decision makers
(0.06)
Had workplace education
0.140***
(0.05)
Constant
-0.126
1.047***
(0.29)
(0.37)
Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses. *** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1.
15
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Table 5
Panel B: IV Estimates of Financial Literacy Impact on Retirement
Planning
Dependent Variable Equals
First Stage
Second Stage
All three correct
OLS
1 if planner (0 else)
GMM
Observations
751
751
R-squared
0.1417
-0.410
F statistic
6.41
3.97
Test of Excluded Instruments
F(5, 727)
4.48
Prob>F
0.0005
Partial R-squared of Excluded
Instruments
0.0268
(the F-statistics for the joint significance of the instruments and the partial R-
squared of the instruments). While significant, the F statistic is small by
conventional standards and indicates that our instrumental variables may be weak.
Similarly the partial R-squared is low at 3%. When instrumental variables are
weak, the IV estimates may be biased and therefore offer little or no improvement
over the OLS estimates (Staiger and Stock 1997). Further, any bias correction
offered by the IV estimator is proportional to the explanatory power of the
instruments in the first-stage regression. While our instruments are only weakly
correlated with financial literacy, tests (Hansen’s J) show that they are
exogenous.
11
The second stage shows a positive and significant coefficient on the
instrumented financial literacy variable. The estimates are large. However, given
the potential weakness in the instruments mentioned earlier we recommend
caution in interpreting these results and want to be careful not to overstate our
findings. That said, our results support that financial literacy may lead to greater
retirement planning. Future work should focus on identifying stronger instruments
to confirm this result.
Discussion and Conclusions
Even though participation in the Australian retirement saving system is mandatory
for almost all workers, virtually everyone has the opportunity to make decisions
about important facets of their retirement savings plan. These include voluntary
additional contributions, changes to investment strategies, changes to insurance
provisions, and choice of plan provider. Even more choices confront retirees, who
have almost unlimited freedom to manage the decumulation phase of retirement.
For the rest of the world, Australia presents an interesting natural experiment
in whether making investment compulsory motivates higher financial literacy: it
11
We used the STATA module IVREG2 to perform the IV analysis (Baum et al. 2007).
16
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
is a developed economy where the retirement savings system ensures that almost
all adults are long-term investors with the opportunity to choose their own
portfolios relatively easily.
Individuals need foundational numeracy, a basic understanding of finance
and some familiarity with the features of plans and products to make sound
financial decisions. An interesting question is whether compelling almost
everyone to join a defined contribution plan improves the population’s financial
literacy over time because people learn from their participation. The results
reported here can provide a benchmark for future comparison work in financial
literacy. So far, after 25 years of mandatory retirement saving, a large minority of
Australians does not know the relevant financial basics nor are they actively
preparing for retirement. In addition, the results highlight certain demographic
groups that are most at risk, including the young, women, the least educated,
those not in the labor force and those not employed. Results for Australia are not
markedly worse, but neither are they better than, other comparable countries
(Bateman et al. 2012) and at riskgroups are similarly populated.
These results raise the obvious question: Why is financial literacy still
relatively low in Australia, and why does the mandatory system not motivate
more people to plan for retirement? Finding the answer to this question is a
challenge for future research. The solution will be essential to any efforts to
develop and test methods for improving financial awareness. While our study
does not provide the answers, we can propose several possible explanations that
should warrant attention in future research. While the observed lack of knowledge
could be a function of the mandatory nature of the Australian retirement system
and the system’s default structure, the fact that financial literacy is not markedly
worse in Australia than in other Anglosphere countries suggests that this is
unlikely to be the case. The existence of a compulsory employer contribution rate
may well encourage many Australians to feel that, since they are following
government policy prescriptions, their retirement is secure and therefore does not
need attention. On the other hand, subject to some caution due to sampling biases,
we find that both literacy and planning continue to improve as people age, unlike
some other similar countries where knowledge advances to middle age and then
begins to decay (Lusardi and Mitchell 2011c). This Australian result may indicate
that eventually people begin taking notice of issues surrounding and acquiring
some skills relating to retirement planning. This may not be surprising as the
interaction of a means-tested public Age Pension and individual choice of
retirement benefit necessitates many Australians of retirement age to navigate
product prospectuses and public benefit information.
Alternatively, individuals may not realize they have a knowledge gap. In our
survey, we asked individuals to assess their own knowledge of finance, and only
14% of our sample considered themselves below average. These findings are
17
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
consistent with the 8% figure reported in a large corporate survey conducted by
the Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited (ANZ 2011). The ANZ
survey also found that participants’ perceived need for further financial education
declined with their self-assessed knowledge. Thus, many Australians may not
realize they need more education when, in fact, they do.
While future research is required to determine whether these explanations are
valid, or if there is an alternative cause for the observed lower levels of financial
literacy, this paper does highlight important deficiencies, as well as reveal a
connection between financial knowledge and retirement planning. Our findings
are similar to those around the world suggesting that more research is needed
regarding methods for educating consumers so that they can make more informed
choices.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful for generous funding. Agnew acknowledges support from
the Wharton Schools Pension Research Council and the Global Financial
Literacy Excellence Center. Bateman and Thorp acknowledge financial support
under ARC DP1093842. The Chair of Finance and Superannuation (Thorp),
University of Technology Sydney (UTS), receives support from the Sydney
Financial Forum (through Colonial First State Global Asset Management), the
New South Wales government, the Association of Superannuation Funds of
Australia, the Industry Superannuation Network, and the Paul Woolley Centre for
the Study of Capital Market Dysfunctionality, UTS. In addition, the authors thank
Pureprofile and the staff of the Centre for the Study of Choice (University of
Technology Sydney) for their generous assistance with the development and
implementation of the Internet survey. Finally, the authors thank Mariya
Theiviasingham and Edward Wei for their excellent research assistance.
References
ABS. See Australian Bureau of Statistics.
Agnew, J. 2013. Australia’s Retirement System: Strengths, Weaknesses, and
Reform. Center for Retirement Research Issue Brief 13-5
http://crr.bc.edu/wp-content/uploads/2013/04/IB_13-5-508.pdf (last accessed
May 6, 2013)
Agnew, J., H. Bateman, and S. Thorp. 2013. Work, money, lifestyle: Plans of
Australian retirees. JASSA: The Finsia Journal of Applied Finance No. 1,
March: 45–50.
18
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
Alessie, R., M. van Rooij, and A. Lusardi. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement
preparation in the Netherlands. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance
10: 527–545. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000461
Almenberg, J., and J. Säve-Söderbergh. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement
planning in Sweden. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance 10: 585
598. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000497
Ameriks, J., A. Caplin, and J. Leahy. 2003. Wealth accumulation and the
propensity to plan. Quarterly Journal of Economics 68: 1007–1047.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/00335530360698487
ANZ. See Australia and New Zealand Banking Commission.
ASIC. See Australian Securities and Investments Commission.
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group. 2011. Adult financial literacy in
Australia: Full report of the results from the 2011 ANZ Survey (December).
ANZ, Melbourne, Victoria.
Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2012. Employee earnings, benefits and trade
union membership (27 April 2012). Canberra.
Australian Securities and Investments Commission. 2011. Financial literacy and
behavioral change, Report 230. Australian Securities and Investments
Commission, Sydney.
http://www.financialliteracy.gov.au/media/218309/financial-literacy-and-
behavioural-change.pdf (accessed September 14, 2012)
———. 2012. Shadow shopping study of retirement advice, Report 279,
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, Sydney.
http://www.asic.gov.au/asic/pdflib.nsf/LookupByFileName/rep279-
published-27-March-2012.pdf/$file/rep279-published-27-March-2012.pdf
(accessed September 14, 2012)
Bateman, H., R. Chomik, and J. Piggott. 2013. Australia’s retirement income
policy. In Ageing in Australia: Challenges and Opportunities, ed. K.
O’Loughlin, C. Browning, and H. Kendig, New York: Springer.
Forthcoming.
Bateman, H., C. Ebling, J. Geweke, J. Louviere, S. Satchell, and S. Thorp. 2012.
Financial competence and expectations formation: Evidence from Australia.
Economic Record 88: 3963.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-
4932.2011.00766.x
Baum, C., M. Schaffer, and S. Stillman. 2007. IVREG2: Stata module for
extended instrumental variables/2SLS, GMM and AC/HAC, LIML, and k-
class regression. Boston College Department of Economics, Statistical
Software Components S425401. Boston, MA.
Behrman, J., O. S. Mitchell, C. Soo, and D. Bravo. 2012. How financial literacy
affects household wealth accumulation. American Economic Review: Papers
& Proceedings 102: 300–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.102.3.300
19
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Bucher-Koenen, T., and A. Lusardi. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement
planning in Germany. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance 10: 565
584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000485
Calvet, L. E., J. Y. Campbell, and P. Sodini. 2009. Measuring the financial
sophistication of households. American Economic Review: Papers &
Proceedings 99: 393–398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.99.2.393
Christelis, D., T. Jappelli, and M. Padula. 2010. Cognitive abilities and portfolio
choice. European Economic Review 54: 18–38.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2009.04.001
Crossan, D., D. Feslier, and R. Hurnard. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement
planning in New Zealand. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance 10:
619–635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000515
Croy, G., P. Gerrans, and C. Speelman. 2010. The role of relevance of domain
knowledge, perceptions of planning importance, and risk tolerance in
predicting savings intentions. Journal of Economic Psychology 31: 860–871.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2010.06.002
Gerrans, P., M. Clark-Murphy, and K. Truscott. 2009. Financial literacy and
superannuation awareness of indigenous Australians: Pilot study results.
Australian Journal of Social Issues 44(4): 417–439.
Gilliland, D., V. Melfi, A. Sikorskii, E. Corcoran, and E. Melfi. 2011.
Quantitative literacy at Michigan State University, 2: Connection to financial
literacy. Numeracy 4(2). http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.4.2.6
Fornero, E., and C. Monticone. 2011. Financial literacy and pension plan
participation in Italy. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance, 10: 547
564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000473
Hastings, J., and O. S. Mitchell. 2011. How financial literacy and impatience
shape retirement wealth and investment behaviors. NBER Working Paper
No. 16740. National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA.
Klapper, L., and G. A. Panos. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement planning:
the Russian case. Journal of Pension Economics and Finance 10: 599–618.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000503
Lusardi, A. 2009. U.S. household savings behavior: The role of financial literacy,
information and financial education programs. In Policymaking insights from
behavioral economics, ed. C. Foote, L. Goette, and S. Meier, 109–149.
Boston, MA: Federal Reserve Bank of Boston.
Lusardi, A., and O. S. Mitchell. 2010. How ordinary consumers make complex
economic decisions: Financial literacy and retirement readiness. NBER
Working Paper w15350, National Bureau of Economic Research,
Cambridge, MA.
———. 2011a. Financial literacy and planning: implications for retirement
wellbeing. In Financial literacy: Implications for retirement security and the
20
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
financial marketplace, ed. A. Lusardi and O. S. Mitchell, 17–39. Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199696819.003.0002
———. 2011b. Financial literacy and retirement planning in the United States.
Journal of Pension Economics and Finance 10: 509–525.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S147474721100045X
———. 2011c. Financial literacy around the world: an overview. Journal of
Pension Economics and Finance 10: 497–508.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000448
Lusardi, A., and P. Tufano. 2009. Debt literacy, financial experiences, and
overindebtedness. NBER Working Paper No. 14808, National Bureau of
Economic Research, Cambridge, MA.
Sekita, S. 2011. Financial literacy and retirement planning in Japan. Journal of
Pension Economics and Finance 10: 637–656.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1474747211000527
Staiger, D., and J. H. Stock. 1997. Instrumental variables regression with weak
instruments. NBER Technical Working Paper No. 151, National Bureau of
Economic Research, Cambridge, MA.
van Rooij, M., A. Lusardi, and R. Alessie. 2011. Financial literacy and stock
market participation. Journal of Financial Economics 101: 449–472.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfineco.2011.03.006
21
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Appendix
Table A1
Survey Sample versus Australian Population (2564 years old retired and non-retired)
Respondent
Population
Australian
Population
Age 2564
Gender
Male
47%
49%
Female
53%
51%
Age
2529 years
16%
13%
3034 years
16%
13%
3539 years
15%
13%
4044 years
13%
13%
4549 years
10%
13%
5054 years
9%
13%
5559 years
9%
11%
6065 years
10%
11%
Work Status
Employed
a
71%
71%
Not employed
6%
3%
Not in the labor force
15%
25%
b
Retired
8%
not broken out
Marital Status
Never married
22%
27%
Divorced/separated
10%
14%
Widowed
2%
2%
Married or long-term relationship
66%
58%
Income
$1$20,799 (i.e., less than $399 a week)
20%
19%
$20,800$51,999 (i.e., $400$999 a week)
31%
32%
$52,000$103,999 (i.e., $1,000$1,999 a week)
36%
27%
$104,000 (i.e., $2,000 a week) or more
9%
9%
Negative or nil income
5%
6%
Not stated
0%
7%
Highest level of Education
High school or less
24%
36%
TAFE (certificate)
23%
21%
TAFE (diploma)
12%
9%
Bachelor’s degree
25%
17%
Graduate degree
16%
7%
Not stated
0%
9%
Note: Source for population statistics: Australian Bureau of Statistics Census of Population and
Housing, 2011.
a
Employed includes full-time workers, part-time workers, and workers classified away from work.
b
Census records only specify those not in the labor force. Also, includes those not stating their labor
force status.
c
Includes those whose education was inadequately described
22
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
Table A2
Older Sample Population versus Older Australian Population (65 years old+)
Respondent
Population
Australian
Population
Age 65+
Gender
Male
60%
46%
Female
40%
54%
Age
6569 years
61%
31%
7074 years
27%
24%
75 years or over
12%
46%
Work Status
Employed
a
14%
11%
Not employed
3%
0.3%
Not in the labor force
b
3%
89%
Retired
80%
not broken out
Marital Status
Never married
2%
5%
Divorced/separated
15%
12%
Widowed
8%
26%
Married or long-term relationship
75%
57%
Income
$1$20,799 (i.e., less than $399/week)
38%
52%
$20,800$51,999 (i.e., $400$999 /week)
47%
27%
$52,000$103,999 (i.e., $1,000$1,999/week)
10%
5%
$104,000 (i.e., $2,000/week) or more
1%
2%
Negative or nil income
4%
3%
Not stated
0%
11%
Highest level of Education
High school or less
45%
54%
TAFE (certificate)
20%
13%
TAFE (diploma)
14%
6%
Bachelor degree
12%
6%
Graduate degree
10%
3%
Not stated
0%
19%
N
102
Note: Source for population statistics: Australian Bureau of Statistics Census of Population and
Housing, 2011.
a
Employed includes full-time workers, part-time workers, and workers classified away from work.
b
Census records only specify those not in the labor force. Also, includes those not stating their labor
force status.
c
Includes those whose education was inadequately described
23
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013
Table A3, Panel A
Description of Variables
Variable Name
Description
Retirement planning question
Planner
Indicator variable that equals one if the respondent answered yes to the question
Have you ever tried to work out how much you need to save for retirement?”
Financial literacy variables
All three correct
Indicator variable that equals one if all the financial literacy questions were
answered correctly, zero otherwise
Total number correct
Continuous variable that equals the sum of the number of financial literacy
questions answered correctly
Inflation correct
Indicator variable that equals one if the inflation question was answered correctly,
zero otherwise
Interest correct
Indicator variable that equals one if the interest question was answered correctly,
zero otherwise
Risk correct
Indicator variable that equals one if the risk question was answered correctly, zero
otherwise
Total number DKs
Continuous variable that equals the sum of the number of do not know answers
Demographic variables
Age
The age in years of the respondent
Age squared
The square of the age variable
Female
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent is a female, zero otherwise
Education, high school or less
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent’s highest level of education
is high school or less, zero otherwise (This is the omitted variable in the
regressions)
Education, TAFE (Certificate)
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent's highest level of education
is a Technical and Further Education (TAFE) certificate, zero otherwise
Education, TAFE (Diploma)
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent's highest level of education
is a Technical and Further Education (TAFE) diploma or advanced diploma, zero
otherwise
Education, bachelor’s degree
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent's highest level of education
is a bachelor degree, zero otherwise
Education, graduate degree
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent's highest level of education
is a graduate degree, zero otherwise
Married
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent is married or in a
committed long-term relationship, otherwise zero (This is the omitted variable in
the regressions)
Single
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent is single, otherwise zero
Divorced or separated
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent is divorced or separated,
zero otherwise
Income less than $20,800
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent earns an annual personal
gross income of less than $20,800, zero otherwise (This is the omitted variable in
the regressions)
Income $20,800$41,599
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent earns an annual personal
gross income between the two values, zero otherwise
Income $41,600$67,599
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent earns an annual personal
gross income between the two values, zero otherwise
Income greater than $67,600
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent earns an annual personal
gross income between the two values, zero otherwise
Homeowner
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent is a homeowner, zero
otherwise
Not working
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent indicates they are not
employed, zero otherwise
Self-employed
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent indicates that they are self-
employed, zero otherwise
24
Numeracy, Vol. 6 [2013], Iss. 2, Art. 7
https://digitalcommons.usf.edu/numeracy/vol6/iss2/art7
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5038/1936-4660.6.2.7
Table A3, Panel B
Description of Variables
Variable Name
Description
Control variables for liquidity constraints and household income shocks
Had income shock
An indicator that equals one if the individual or someone in the respondent's family
experienced a drastic and unexpected fall in savings or income
Number of children
Number of children in the household
Instrumental variables
Sibling in Worse Situation
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent answered that their oldest sibling
had a worse financial situation, zero otherwise
Sibling in Better Situation
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent answered that their oldest sibling
had a better financial situation, zero otherwise
Parents are good financial
decision makers
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent judged their parent to have an
intermediate or high understanding of financial matters (4 or greater on a 7 point scale).
Exact question in footnote 10.
Don't know if parents are
good financial decision
makers
An indicator variable that equals one if the respondent indicated that they did not know
if their parent had an understanding of financial matters. Exact question in footnote 10.
Had workplace education
An indicator variable that equals on if the respondent answered yes to the question "Did
any of the firms you have worked for (including your current employer) offer financial
education programs such as retirement seminars?"
25
Agnew et al.: Financial Literacy and Retirement Planning in Australia
Published by Digital Commons @ University of South Florida, 2013