University of Kentucky University of Kentucky
UKnowledge UKnowledge
University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School
2003
THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING
Jennifer Sue Shank
University of Kentucky
Right click to open a feedback form in a new tab to let us know how this document bene;ts you. Right click to open a feedback form in a new tab to let us know how this document bene;ts you.
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Shank, Jennifer Sue, "THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING" (2003).
University of Kentucky
Doctoral Dissertations
. 397.
https://uknowledge.uky.edu/gradschool_diss/397
This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at UKnowledge. It has been
accepted for inclusion in University of Kentucky Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of
UKnowledge. For more information, please contact UKnowledge@lsv.uky.edu.
ABSTRACT OF DISSERTATION
Jennifer Sue Shank
The Graduate School
University of Kentucky
2003
THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING
_____________________________
ABSTRACT OF DISSERTATION
_____________________________
A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the
Requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy
in the College of Fine Arts
at The University of Kentucky
By
Jennifer Sue Shank
Lexington, Kentucky
Director: Dr. Cecilia Wang, Associate Professor of Music
Lexington, Kentucky
2003
Copyright ©Jennifer Sue Shank 2003
ABSTRACT OF DISSERTATION
THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of visual stimuli on music
listening skills in pre-service elementary teachers. “Visual Stimuli” in this study refers to
the presentation of arts elements in selected visually projected images of paintings.
Music listening skills are defined as those skills needed to identify and interpret musical
excerpts. A Pretest-Posttest Control-group Design was used in this study.
Subjects were pre-service elementary general educators enrolled in a large
southern university (N=93). Students from intact classes were randomly placed into
either the experimental group or the control group. The treatment consisted of six music
listening lessons over a two-week period with each group receiving the identical teaching
protocol with the exception of the use of paintings with the experimental group.
Listening instruction emphasized the identification of melodic contour, instrumentation,
texture, rhythm and expressive elements of the compositions.
The Teacher Music Listening Skills Test (TMLST) was constructed by the
investigator and administered before and after the treatment. The TMLST was designed
to assess music listening skills in adult non-musicians.
Results indicate that the group receiving visual stimuli in the form of paintings
scored significantly higher on listening skills (p<.01) than the control group which
received no visual stimuli in the form of visually projected images of paintings. There
was an instruction effect on both preference and familiarity of the musical pieces for both
the control group and the experimental group.
KEY WORDS: VISUAL STIMULI, MUSIC LISTENING SKILLS, PAINTINGS, NON-
MUSICIANS, INSTRUMENTAL MUSIC, GESTALT
Jennifer S. Shank
July, 20 2003
THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING
By
Jennifer Sue Shank
Dr. Cecilia C. Wang
Director of Dissertation
Dr. Lance W. Brunner
Director of Graduate Studies
RULES FOR USE OF DISSERTATION
Unpublished dissertation submitted for the Doctor’s degree and deposited in the
University of Kentucky Library are as a rule open for inspection, but are to be used with
due regard to the rights of the authors. Bibliographical references may be noted, but
quotations or summaries of parts may be published only with the permission of the
author, and with the usual scholarly acknowledgements.
Extensive copying or publication of the thesis in whole or in part requires also the
consent of the Dean of the Graduate School of the University of Kentucky.
A library which borrows this thesis for use by its patrons is expected to secure the
signature of each user.
DISSERTATION
Jennifer Sue Shank
The Graduate School
University of Kentucky
2003
THE EFFECT OF VISUAL ART ON MUSIC LISTENING
______________________________
DISSERTATION
_____________________________
A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the
Requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy
in the College of Fine Arts at
The University of Kentucky
By
Jennifer Sue Shank
Lexington, Kentucky
Director:
Dr. Cecilia Wang, Associate Professor of Music
Lexington, Kentucky
2003
Copyright © Jennifer Sue Shank, 2003
For my Parents
ACKNOWELDGMENTS
I would like to thank all the people that made the impossible possible. Thank you to Bill
and Lisa, for always being there. Thank you to my grandparents, Marge Jossi and August
Keyerleber, who have supported me and made sure there was food on the table and a
warm place to sleep. Thank you to Dr. Cecilia Wang, whose mentoring, guidance and
friendship were invaluable throughout my education. To Dr. David Sogin who has
helped me grow from a scared new student to a confident graduate. To Dr. Kate
Covington, Dr. Ron Pen and Dr. Skip Kifer for agreeing to sit on my committee. To my
colleagues, April McAllister and Donna Irwin, to whom I am eternally grateful for all of
the help, support and graded papers. And finally, thank you to my parents, Janet and Bill
Shank, for always encouraging me and allowing me to be anything in life.
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS………………………………………………………………..………iii
LIST OF TABLES..………………………………………………………………………………vi
LIST OF FIGURES…..………………………………………………….………………………vii
LIST OF FILES………………………………………………………….……………………...viii
Chapter One: Introduction………………………...………………………………………………1
Chapter Two: Related Literature…....…………………………………………………………….3
Part One: On Listening……………………………………………………………………3
The Basis of Listening….…………………………………………………………3
Processing of Sound During Listening……………………………………………5
Listening for Understanding………………………………………………………7
Listening Lesson Approaches……………………………………………………..9
Aesthetic Education Through Listening…….……………………………..……..10
Listeners with Different Learning Styles….……………………………………..15
Differences Among Listeners...………………………………………………….17
Motivation for Listening…………………………………………………………18
Part Two: On Visual Stimuli…………………………………………………………….20
Visual Stimuli and Instruction…………………………………………………...20
Historical Connection Between Visual Art and Music…………….……………22
Art in Music Education…………………………………………………………..24
Applications of Gestalt Principles in Art and Music…………………………….24
Memory and Listening…...……………………………………………………...29
Statement of the Hypothesis……………………………………………………………..31
Chapter Three: Methodology and Introduction………..………………………………………...32
Selection of Subjects……………………………………………………………………..32
Research Design………………………………………………………………………….33
Instrumentation…………………………………………………………………………..35
Procedure………………………………………………………………………………...37
Treatment………………………………………………………………………………...39
Music and Art Used in the Study………………………………………………………...40
iv
Chapter Four: Results……………………………………………………………………………45
Results Related to Listening Skills………………………………………..……………..46
Results of Hypothesis Testing………………………………………………..…….……53
Secondary Results……………………………………………………………..…………53
Summary…………………………………………………………………………………57
Chapter Five: Discussion and Recommendations………………………………………………..57
Recommendations for Further Research….……………………………………….……..64
Implications for Educational Practice..…………………………………………………..65
Appendices………………………………………………………………………………………67
Appendix A- TMLST……………………………………………………………………67
Appendix B- Recall Test…………………………………………………………………77
Appendix C- Permission…………………………………………………………………83
Appendix D- IRB………………………………………………………………………...84
Appendix E- Sample Listening Lesson Scripts …….……..………………………….…85
Appendix F- Concepts Taught During Listening Lessons………………………….……87
Appendix G-Art Survey………………………………………………………………….90
Appendix H- Pilot Study…………………………………………………………………92
References………………………………………………………………………………………106
Vita……………………………………………………………………………………………...117
v
LIST OF TABLES
Table 4.1 Participant Demographic Information………………………………………………...45
Table 4.2 Table of Means for Pre-test TMLST for Subgroups 1-8……………….……………..46
Table 4.3 ANOVA Summary of TMLST for Pre-test Subgroups 1-8…...………………………47
Table 4.4 Table of Means for Pre-test Part B for Classes 1-4…..……………...………………..47
Table 4.5 ANOVA Summary of TMLST for Class Sections 1-4…...…………………………...48
Table 4.6 ANOVA Summary for Pre-test………...……….……………………………………..48
Table 4.7 Mean Values of TMLST Part A..……………………………………………………..49
Table 4.8 Mean Values of TMLST Part B…...…………………………………………………..49
Table 4.9 TMLST Means Showing Improvement from Pre-test to Post-test..…………………..50
Table 4.10 ANOVA Summary of TMLST Post-test for the Experimental and Control Groups..50
Table 4.11 Table of Means for TMLST Post-test for Subgroups 1-8…………………………...51
Table 4.12 Table of Means Shows Improvement from Pre-test to Post-test Divided by Subgroups
1-8………………………………………………………………………………………………..52
Table 4.13 Means of TMLST Post-test per Excerpt Score for Each Category…………………..54
Table 4.14 Mean Rating for Preference and Familiarity…………………….…………………..55
Table 4.15 Significant Correlations………………….…………………………………………..56
vi
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 2.1 Common Decibel Ranges……………………………………………………………...4
Figure 2.2 The Ear………………………………………………………………………………...5
Figure 2.3 Wlodkoski’s Time Continuum Model for Motivation……………………………….18
Figure 2.4 Music and Association with Gestalt Laws…………………………………………...28
Figure 3.1 Research Study Design Model.…….…...……………………………………………34
Figure 3.2 Music Used in the Study……………………………………………………………...41
Figure 3.3 Paintings Used in this Study….……………………………………………………...42
Figure 3.4 Art and Music Combined, Direct and Indirect Relationships……………………..…42
vii
LIST OF FILES
Dissertation, Jennifer S. Shank, The Univerisity of Kentucky………………………………645kb
viii
1
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Listening to music is prerequisite to all other musical pursuits. Focus of attention
combined with developing a high level of aural discrimination seems to provide the basis
for meaningful music listening. Listening contributes to musical understanding and
enjoyment as well as increasing one’s aesthetic sensitivity (Madsen, C. & Geringer, J.M.
2000). The ability to listen is the first and most important skill that is needed for all
musical activities. Haack (1992) states,
Listening is the fundamental skill. Some aestheticians argue or imply that
until sounds are heard and perceived as music, there is no music. Clearly
this is the practical truth as concerns music listening. Music exists for
hearing and listening. Such listening is a skill in and of itself, as well as a
vital part of all other musical skills. Yet music listening is among the last
and least studied aspects of music (p. 451).
The National Standards for Arts Education (Consortium of National Arts Education
Associations, 1994) states that “proficient” students in grades 9-12 should be able to
“identify and explain compositional devices and techniques used to provide unity and
variety and tension and release in a musical work”(p. 61). “Advanced” students should
be able to “analyze and describe uses of the elements of music in a given work that make
it unique, interesting and expressive” (p.61). In order to accomplish these tasks students
must be able to hear the interaction between the elements of music that the composer has
used and be able to recall them.
People who are not musically trained approach listening differently than trained
musicians do. The non-trained person listens to features such as texture and melody
before other aspects of a musical piece. Previous research has indicated that adults learn
better when they are presented with materials in a multi-sensory way. This would
suggest that the use of paintings to reinforce musical knowledge would enhance the
listening skills of college students.
We compare the arts in order to discover similarities and common elements and to
draw parallels between them. Aristotle’s influential categorization in The Poetics of
2
painting, music and poetry as imitative arts was an element in his quest for a unified
theory of aesthetics. The need to compare also comes from the frequent inadequacy or
failure of the aesthetic language. Whenever critics and aestheticians have found
themselves struggling to describe something they have perceived in a work of poetry,
music or visual art, they have often fallen back on expressions such as the “poetry of
painting, the painting of poetry or the poetry of music,” attempting to characterize
something difficult to define by referring it to something else that is difficult to define.
The hope, presumably, is that some aspect of one art form will help to illuminate some
aspect of the other (Kagan, 1986). This hope fuels the idea of using art to help
understand music. Art and music are interrelated and influential upon each other for
inspiration, shared meaning and symbolic representation.
In the present school curriculum, we expect our classroom teachers to integrate
the arts into teaching different subjects and one or two courses in teaching music are
usually required college courses for teacher preparation programs. When teachers are
asked to integrate music into a general education classroom they are expected to make
listening an integral part of that, yet, there is very little research to indicate what method
is effective in training the listening skills of future teachers.
The purpose of this study is to investigate whether using the projected images of
paintings will enhance the ability of pre-service teaching candidates to identify and
recognize musical features of particular musical excerpts. If awareness of similarities in
the visual stimuli serves as a catalyst for awareness of similarities in the auditory stimuli,
this would indicate that visual art should be employed for pedagogical purposes in the
future.
3
CHAPTER TWO
RELATED LITERATURE
This chapter contains a review of literature on listening and visual stimuli. In
examining listening, it is important to look at how sound is created and internalized by
the ear and brain, how humans process sound, and how people understand that for which
they listen. Other important elements to consider are approaches in listening lessons,
aesthetic education and listening, listeners with different learning styles, and the
motivation for listening. The subject of visual stimuli is a broad topic that can be
examined in a multitude of ways. For this study, the aspects of visual stimuli that are
most important are those that directly affect how individuals process what they see and
how they process it in conjunction with other stimuli. Factors in this research include
visual stimuli and instruction, connecting visual art and music, applications of Gestalt
principles in art and music, and memory and learning.
Part One
On Listening
The Basis of Listening
The capacity to perceive music and respond at both a cognitive and an affective
level can be learned through particular listening experiences. One of the goals of music
education is to present strategies which will encourage attention to the music and enable
the listener to form cognitive and affective opinions on a piece. In order for these
activities to be effective, active engagement is necessary. To engage a listener, activities
and lessons must be developed that enhance understanding and allow the listener to
organize information in a meaningful way.
Even before examining how to approach active listening, one must consider how
one listens. It is the sensory experience of hearing that makes the perception of music
possible. The ability for individuals to use their hearing for the purpose of listening
varies. Good hearing does not necessarily insure skilled listening and, conversely, poor
4
hearing does not indicate an inability to listen. While good hearing is not completely
dependent on one’s ability to listen well, hearing does play a significant role in the
perception of music (Darrow, 1990). Listening is a mental process; the act of hearing is a
physical one. Understanding the physical act of hearing can offer insight into the human
potential for the capacity to listen and, ultimately, to understand. Examination of the
components of the aural process can provide general guidelines for structuring and
sequencing listening experiences. Sound consists of vibrations that travel in waves,
generally through air. Sound waves travel at different rates of speed; the faster the waves
travel the higher the pitch. Frequency is the measured number of vibrations per second
and is represented in hertz (Lipscomb & Hodges, 1996). Pitch is a subjective judgment,
while frequency is the physical reality of the speed of the sound wave. Normal hearing
range is 20-20,000 Hz. Intensity of sound is the amount of energy within a sound wave.
Intensity can be measured quantitatively in decibels, but loudness is a subjective
measurement of sound. Intensity is measured in decibels (dB). Zero decibels are the
quietest sound. Sound that exceeds 120 decibels can cause pain (Lipscomb & Hodges,
1996). Common decibel ranges are presented in the chart below.
Figure 2.1 Common Decibel Levels
Decibel Levels Sound Source Musical Level
0dB just audible sound
30dB soft whisper background music
50dB normal conversation mp
60dB loud conversation mf
80dB shouting f
90dB shouting marching band
5
The ear is divided into three parts: outer, middle and inner ear. (See figure below)
Figure 2.2 The Ear
Sound waves enter the external canal of the outer ear, strike the eardrum and cause the
eardrum to vibrate. The vibrations from the eardrum reach the three bones of the middle
ear, the malleus, incus and stapes. These bones carry the sound waves across the middle
ear to the inner ear. Within the inner ear are the semicircular canals and the cochlea.
The semicircular canals are filled with fluid and lined with hairs and are responsible for
balance and equilibrium. The chochlea is involved in hearing. Within the cochlea lies
the Organ of Corti which is covered with many fine hairs. Sounds of different
frequencies affect hair cells at different locations. The thinner, shorter hairs, near the
opening of the Organ of Corti, respond to high sounds. The thicker, longer hairs, farthest
from the opening, pick up low sounds. Intensity is determined primarily by how many
hair cells are affected. The more hairs that are made to vibrate by a sound wave the
louder the sound is perceived. The auditory nerve then receives impulses from these
hairs and carries it to the hearing center (auditory cortex) within the brain. Once the
brain assumes control of the process, hearing becomes listening.
Processing of Sound During Listening
The brain is responsible for the levels of discrimination that we make during
listening. Training the brain to listen requires several elements: analysis of the desired
auditory task, the structuring of successive approximations to the desired goal, and
regular and systematic evaluation of auditory level. According to Norman P. Erber and
6
Ira J. Hirsh (1978), auditory tasks can be broken into four basic levels of aural
processing: (Erber & Hirsh, 1978).
1. Detection, in which the listener determines the presence or absence of
specified sound stimuli.
2. Discrimination, in which the listener perceives differences in sound stimuli
such as loud and soft or high and low.
3. Identification, in which the listener appropriately applies labels to the sounds
4. Comprehension, in which the listener makes critical judgments regarding the
sound stimuli.
There are a number of other listening behaviors that can be subsumed within the four
basic levels of the auditory processing mentioned above. In 1977, Derek Sanders
developed a hierarchy of auditory processing for speech. Since both speech and music
have similar properties, Sanders’ hierarchy can be applied to music. The following is that
hierarchy with reference to application in music:
1. Awareness of acoustic stimuli.
2. Localization: Can the listener identify the location of the sound source?
3. Attention: Can the listener attend to music over time?
4. Discrimination between speech and non speech or chant and melody.
5. Auditory discrimination: Can the listener discriminate between the timbres of
different instruments; can he or she locate the entrance and exit of specific
instruments within the total music context?
6. Suprasegmental discrimination: can the listener make discriminations about the
expressive qualities of the music?
7. Segmental discrimination: can the listener make discriminations about pitch?
8. Auditory memories: can the listener remember what instruments were heard?
9. Auditory sequential memory: can the listener remember in what order
instruments were heard?
10. Auditory syntheses: can the listener make critical judgments regarding form,
texture and harmony?
According to Sanders (1977), suprasegmental discrimination is defined as discrimination
on a large scale of such things as texture, form and expressive elements and segmental
7
discrimination is defined as discrimination of musical cues such as pitch. Sanders, along
with the other research, suggest that there are steps to listening and the process of
listening. This lends itself to what takes place during a listening activity in a music
education class such as the one used for this research. Students begin with an awareness
of acoustic stimuli and work their way through the hierarchy to achieve auditory memory.
Students are presented with a new musical piece to listen to and they are directed as to
what to listen for. Students make discriminations whether there are voices or
instrumental only, then different timbres, instrumentation and entrances and exits of
instruments. After students have made discriminations on timbre and instrumentation,
students then make critical decisions on expressive qualities.
Listening for Understanding
Once the hearing process involves mental processing, hearing becomes listening.
Successful listening is an active process that enhances understanding of the sound.
Listening is temporal because it involves making sense of information that is never all
presented at the same time. Understanding of aural events depends on what a listener
retains in a continuous moving stream of information (Elliott, D. J. 1995). Processes
involved in listening to music include sensory transduction, auditory grouping, analysis
of auditory properties and features, and matching immediate sonic events with an
auditory lexicon of previously experienced sounds (McAdams, S. 1993). For a listener
to deduce meaning and understanding from music, the music must create a meaningful
connection to that listener. Elliott (1995) points out that listening to music is analogous
to constructing a moving jigsaw puzzle. Listeners do not simply listen from wholes-to-
parts or parts-to-wholes because auditory parts and wholes coalesce (p.84). Green (1988)
describes the experience of listening to music as the result of the interaction between our
perceptions of the inherent meaning of sound (structural elements) and the degree to
which the sounds delineate themselves as sounds which are meaningful to us. This
would suggest that for listening to be active and successful, the musical sounds must be
familiar in some ways and must be associative with meaningful representation. In other
words, the sounds must have familiar timbres or instrument sounds and those sounds
must match our socially defined concepts about music.
8
A person who has experienced many ways of organizing particular types of
stimuli is at an advantage when confronted by unfamiliar materials that need organization
(Tait and Haack, 1984). The importance of segmentation in music cognition has been
emphasized in several models of music perception including that of Lerdahl &
Jackendoff (1983) and empirical studies by Krumhansl (1996). Listeners regularly
exploit melodic cues to recognize and to distinguish between different pieces of music
(Rosner & Meyer, 1986). It has also been suggested that anchoring can be effective for
the listening experience. Anchoring, as shown in research by Povel and Jansen (2001),
appears to be a powerful tool for tracing perceptual mechanisms at work in the on-line
processing of music. Anchoring, as was first described by Bharucha (1994), links
unstable tones to stable tones and gives the listener something to focus on within the
melodic structure. Musical experience is meaningful for the listener when the listener is
able to employ information that is familiar to glean understanding. Listening is not a
passive process; there is a difference between simple reception and active construction.
Certain cognitive abilities must be used in order to perceive the sound signals by the ear
and interpret them as music. Acoustical properties of music are organized by the mind
and then associations and connections are made (Mullee, 1996). This information would
suggest that carefully planned listening experiences may lead to a mode of attending that
is both different and more fulfilling than the free associational thinking that listening to
music so often involves. It is possible to increase the likelihood of people actually
engaging in works of art, moving inside of them through acts of imagination, and
perceiving them against their own personal histories as meaningful (Greene, 1986).
Choosing appropriate features for which to listen must be considered. When we
listen to music we actively select salient features from the stream of musical sounds,
focusing our attention one minute on one part of the sound environment and the next
moment on another. Attentional focus is guided by knowledge structures, or schemata,
developed through past experience (Dowling, W.J, & Harwood, D. W,1986. and Neisser
U. 1976). Listeners focus on what is recognizable first, relying on what they have
learned or developed in the past before moving to the new or unrecognizable. Because
listeners seek out salient features that are recognizable it is important to consider what
musical elements should be the focus in this study.
9
For this research the following musical elements will receive focus during
listening activities: melodic contour, texture, beat structure, articulation rhythm and
instrumentation. Research indicates that listeners’ mental representations of novel
melodies contain contour information but relatively little information about absolute pitch
or exact interval size (Dowling, W. J. 1994). Dowling also states that listeners make
errors about interval and absolute pitch of novel melodies soon after they are presented.
By contrast, listeners retain contour information for longer periods of time. This would
indicate that students would be more successful listening for melodic contour rather than
direct melodies. With regard to instrumentation, Rentz (1992) suggests that non-
musicians pay less attention to tone colors of strings while selecting the more obvious
tone color of brass, percussion and woodwind instruments. For this study, music was
chosen to reflect Rentz’s research. Beat structure, articulation and texture were chosen
based on the model by Sanders (1977). Sanders suggests that the use of suprasegmental
discrimination and segmental discrimination allows the listener to pick out global
concepts such as form and musical elements such as texture. He also suggests that the
highest level in his hierarchy would allow the listener to make critical judgments
regarding form and texture.
Listening Lesson Approaches
Carefully planned listening experiences may lead to a mode of attending that is
both different and more fulfilling than the reverie and free association thinking that
casual listening to music so often involves. Often, these planned listening experiences
are organized by following listening guides. The purpose of a listening guide is to focus
specifically on what to listen for in music. Strategies for planned listening experiences
include presenting a systematic method of music analysis, examining musical styles and
treatment of musical structure during different chronological periods, and exploring
different forms of music. Some of these approaches include The Experience of Music
(Reimer, 1972). The book explores the creative process of music, examining the
aesthetic sensibilities of the composer, the performer and the listener. Reimer also
discusses music by categorizing the structural elements: Rhythm, Harmony, Melody,
Tone Color, and Form. Reimer has also created a set of listening lessons to accompany
10
his 1972 text, Developing the Experience of Music: Listening Charts (1973). This text
lays out a concise plan for teaching students to listen by dividing lessons by musical
element. The listening charts are designed to focus on one musical element at a time.
For example, there are eight listening lessons that focus on form with different musical
examples for each. A Concise Introduction to Music Listening (Hoffer, 1979) suggests a
different approach that is more segmented into basic elements, musical form, and musical
types, western and nonwestern. Chapters are not laid out by musical element; instead the
text is laid out to follow an order of what students should know to be able to listen
effectively. Chapters include instruction on how music is written down, how to classify a
piece of music and instructions on how to listen.
Each text and listening guide is concerned with changing the listener’s perception
and origination of various elements in music. Both of these texts offer a great deal of
information, but for listeners who are not knowledgeable in music and who may not be
familiar with what they are listening for, these guides may be overwhelming or
discouraging to a listener. The approach favored by this researcher is to promote
listening skills by using materials familiar to the listeners in a multi-sensory setting.
Aesthetic Education Through Listening
The ability to detect aesthetic form (the arrangement of elements that attracts,
holds and directs the interest of the listener) is at the heart of music education (Broudy,
1958). This ability is needed to be successful at any musical skill, listening included.
Schwadron (1967) states that meaning in music is connected with the uniqueness of the
organization and control of sound, notated by symbols and characterized by the
relationships of music to the human senses and intellect. Music combines formal
elements such as melody, harmony, rhythm, and timbre into aesthetically patterned
sounds that have symbolic meaning in a culture of a society and its individuals (Mullee,
1996). The quality of a musical experience depends upon the type of connection that
occurs between the perception of musical stimuli and the responses to musical stimuli.
Sound emotion is a single musical concept and cultural associations are related in
listening to a performance (Masterson, 1994). Green (1988) describes musical
experience as a result of the interaction between our perception of the inherent meaning
11
of sound and the degree to which these sounds delineate themselves as sounds which are
meaningful to us. In other words, do they match our socially defined concepts about
music? This is particularly relevant in listening situations which present music to which
that the listener is unaccustomed.
In experiencing the combination of inherent and delineated meaning of an
unfamiliar musical experience, the capacity to actively engage in musical situations gains
insight into one’s self (musical or otherwise) and into the relationship of one’s self to
one’s own and other musical cultures. Accompanying all such risk-taking, disorientation
and eventual musical acculturation is self-examination and the personal reconstruction of
one’s relationships, assumptions and performances (Elliott, 1990).
The active contribution that listeners make in the aesthetic situation should not be
underestimated. Levinson (1990) states that we can feel emotion and recognize its
expression in the structure of tones in music. Both the emotion and musical structures
reference one another to heighten the expressiveness of the musical focus. Even strictly
formalist theories recognize that art, specifically music, is never self contained. Roger
Fry (1920) admits that art causes emotional responses based on our physical and
psychological traits. Although the basis for awareness in humans lies in the perception of
the senses, the scope of aesthetic experiences requires us to expand the definition of
perception to include the realms of imagination, fantasy, memory and dreams. This type
of inclusion of the perceptual experience is central to music and all of the arts. Dowling
and Harwood (1986) have found that listeners find it quite natural to attach general
emotional labels to pieces of music.
Dewey (1958) recognized the concept of total organic involvement in art - the
biological, the constant rhythm that marks the interaction of the live creature with his
surroundings (p. 15). This underlies his philosophy of experience including art:
It is proof that man uses the materials and energies of nature with intent to
expand his own life and he does so in accord with the structure of his own
organism- brain, sense-organs and muscular system. Art is the living and
concrete proof that man is capable of restoring consciously and thus on the
plane of meaning, the union of sense, need, impulse and action
12
characteristic of the live creature. The intervention of consciousness adds
regulation, power of selection and predisposition (p.25).
Suzanne Langer (1957) maintains that art possesses the form of living things and that
artistic forms are symbolic of human feeling. Art embodies the form of experience-what
life feels like. She makes the distinction between discursive forms of symbolization
which communicate meanings in an unambiguous manner and presentational symbols
such as those used in the arts which communicate metaphorically, where the symbol or
symbols must be seen “as a whole” rather than divided into individual meanings.
Berleant (1991), advocating a participatory aesthetic, emphasizes replacing
disinterestedness with engagement and contemplation with participation. He outlines the
four principal aspects of the aesthetic situation: the creative, the objective, the
appreciative and the performative. In his view, “music exemplifies the creative aspect of
perception; the composer’s activity in generating musical materials that is paralleled by
both the performer and the listener” (p.5). Music as a performing art is unique in that it
exists in time and needs to be recreated by both performers and listeners. Clarke (1989)
recognizes this, stating that musical events and the way in which they are performed,
perceived and created by performer and listeners give greater recognition to the natural
relationship that characterizes an organism and its environment.
All of these views of aesthetic experience share commonalties in that they reflect
an enlarging of aesthetic experience beyond a particular act of consciousness or
disinterested contemplation of a separate aesthetic object. It is the capacity to respond to
these properties that concerns us as educators. How does one change perception or
understanding of deeper intrinsic meaning? How does a teacher facilitate receptivity?
A place to start is with philosophers such as Langer and Goodman, who have both
written extensively on symbolization. Goodman (1968) continues Langer’s line of
thinking regarding the difference between discursive and presentational symbol systems.
He examines the psychological and educational implications of different kinds of
symbolic competencies:
“Once the arts and sciences are seen to involve working with- inventing,
applying, reading, transforming, manipulating, - symbol systems that
13
agree and differ in certain specific ways, we can perhaps undertake
pointed psychological investigations of how the pertinent skills inhibit or
enhance one another; and the outcome might well call for changes in
educational technology” (p.265).
Goodman (1968) calls attention to a range of symbolic codes such as language,
gesture, and musical notation. Human artistry is viewed as an activity of the mind, an
activity that involves the use and transformation of various symbols and symbol systems.
Individuals who wish to participate meaningfully in artistic perception must learn to
decode the various artistic symbols in their culture; individuals who wish to participate in
artistic creation must learn how to manipulate those symbols. Just as one cannot assume
that in the absence of help and support individuals will learn to read and write in their
natural language, one can assume that individuals can benefit from assistance in learning
to “read” and “write” in the various languages of the arts (Gardner, 1990, p.9). That
being said, a music program must address this and students must learn to understand the
symbols presented during a listening experience. Information is presented during the
performance of a piece of music and students should have the tools needed to participate
meaningfully.
The task in music education has been to discover truths about music, musical
behavior, and cognitive and affective links so that the aesthetic experience might be
identified, purused and developed in the proper educational setting (Schwadron, 1984,
p.17). Broudy (1958) maintains that the place of music in a specific curriculum should
be based on aesthetic considerations. Leonhard and House (1972) also write that the
primary purpose of music education is to develop the aesthetic potential possessed by
every person to its highest level. They believe aesthetic education satisfies our basic
need for symbolic experience and provides a means for self-realization and insight
(p.115). Reimer (1989) has constructed teaching models supporting his view that insights
from aesthetics, when incorporated with the expertise of musicians and educators, can
help articulate the values of the music experience and, specifically, listening.
Bowman (1969) calls for an aesthetic–based type of musical listening instruction.
He writes, “to engage in criticism as instructional method is to guide students away from
14
snap judgments, to direct them toward preferences grounded in closely scrutinized value
systems undergirded by the fullest possible musical awareness, to foster ultimate
sensitivity for the considered views of others and a willingness to entertain alternative
perspectives and to develop a tolerance for variousness and difficulty” (p.12). Reimer
(1993) states that “music education must concern itself with both the diversity and depth
of quality of the musical experience” (p.21). “Music is a universal, human phenomenon,
yet at the same time, is a manifestation of a particular cultural belief system about how
sound should properly be made into music” (p. 24).
The position of many music education theorists is consistent with the position set
forth by the Getty Foundation and the National Endowment for the Arts: arts education
should include history, aesthetics and criticism, in addition to performance or production.
Unfortunately, emphasis in many music programs is almost entirely performance-based,
an emphasis that has generated support from parents and administrators (Leonhard,
1991p.203). Performance programs have done great things for both the profession and
the students involved in them. However, general music programs, despite the profession
recommendations, have not fared as well. “Many college students claim to have missed
out on having music available to them in elementary school and others regret not having
taken some music course beyond elementary school” (Drago, 1993 p. 40). Bresler (1993)
argues that the goals of music education are agreed upon, yet there exists a gap between
desired and actual outcomes. Although perception is the basis of musical experience
(Campbell, 1991, p35), the average listener receives no instruction in categorizing
musical phenomena. Most music is listened to and filtered through self-created and often
biased categories (Cutietta, 1993, p.52).
The literature regarding aesthetics in music education suggests that music
educators and administrators seem to agree that the development of listening skills should
rank highly as a concept in a music curriculum and that understanding what one listens to
is valuable to developing abilities for listening. Active listening is something most
people are capable of learning yet it does not receive the focus or attention due to a
variety of reasons, including time and budgetary constraints. Because of these
constraints, it is all the more important to teach general educators how to listen and how
to teach listening effectively.
15
Listeners with Different Learning Styles
People learn in different ways. In order to have a better understanding of how
people learn it is valuable to look at the process by which people learn and the mode
people use to learn most effectively. Learning style theory has its roots in the
psychoanalytic community (Silver, Strong and Perini, 1997). Carl Jung (1927) was the
father of learning style theory in that he noted the differences in the way students
perceived, made decisions and interacted, and how active or reflective they were while
interacting. Katherine Briggs and Isabel Meyers (1977) created the Myers-Briggs Type
Indicator, applied Jung’s work and influenced 25 years of research in this area.
Educators have become aware of the research of cognitive and educational psychologists
in the area of individual differences and learning styles. The research of these
psychologists in the area of learning styles, following the lead of Jung and Myers and
Briggs, includes that by Grasha and Reichmann, (1975); Hill, (1976); Dunn and Dunn,
(1978); Kolb, (1981); Gregorc, (1982); Silver and Hanson, (1995). Although theories in
learning style interpret the personality in different ways, nearly all models have two
things in common: a focus on process and an emphasis on personality (Silver, Strong &
Perini, 1997). Each of these theories provides educators with additional insights into how
to work with a diverse population of learners.
Learning styles are broadly described as cognitive, affective and physiological
traits that are relatively stable indicators of how learners perceive, interact with and
respond to the learning environment (Keefe, 1979). More specifically, style refers to a
pervasive quality in the learning strategies or the learning behavior of an individual, “a
quality that persists though content may change” (Fischer and Fischer, 1979, p. 245).
Learning style can also be defined as a biological developmental set of personal
characteristics that make the identical instruction effective for some students and
ineffective for others (Dunn and Dunn, 1993). Awareness of these different learning
strengths allows educators to customize lessons and presentations using multiple
modalities.
Research with secondary education students shows that teaching should address
all types of learning styles and use multi-sensory instructional materials, thus providing
16
resources and alternatives to assist students in gaining mastery of the curriculum (Park,
2000). This research would suggest that given how adults process learning, using a
multi-sensory approach to a concept would be more effective than using one mode of
teaching and one learning style.
Humans are typically visually oriented and the retention of information
presented in a visual form usually exceeds retention of information presented verbally
(Levie & Lentz, 1982). Being visually oriented however is typically not enough.
Students should have Visual Literacy, the ability to interpret visual messages accurately
along with the ability to create such messages (Rakes & Rakes, 1995). Research suggests
that the appropriate use of relevant visuals can enhance recall and understanding of
material, increase interest and motivation, and promote critical thinking (Blatnik, 1988,
Pressley and Miller, 1987 and Issing et al, 1989). Many studies demonstrate that visual
learning can positively affect cognitive process such as recall and problem solving
(Anglin, 1986; Ritchey, 1982; Yang and Wedman, 1993). Combining one or more
learning style also enhances learning. A breadth of processing occurs when identical
content is used in two different forms. This then can lead to better memory because
understanding of one form is likely to improve understanding through the other form
(Craik & Tulving, 1975). Using more than one sensory modality and instructional
materials with dual mode presentation, (e.g. visual diagram accompanied by an auditory
text) can be more efficient than the equivalent single modality formats (Kalyuga, S,
Chandler, P & Sweller, J, 2000). Also, the amount of information that can be processed
using both auditory and visual channels can be considerably larger than that using only a
single channel (Kalyuga, S, Chandler, P & Sweller, J, 2000). Therefore, it can be
assumed that a treatment that actively used both visual and auditory modes for learning a
concept would be effective for more learners. It also suggests that using two learning
modalities allows the learner to use a representation that may not directly explain a
concept but in some way enhances it. A picture that does not directly explain a concept
can provide a visual representation in memory to which the student can link supporting
ideas (McDaneil & Pressley, 1987). This would lend itself well to using visual art to
assist students in listening concepts of musical compositions. For this research, the
listening lessons will employ two modalities, the visual and the aural modalities.
17
Differences Among Listeners
Previous research findings indicate that listeners use different strategies in
listening. Since the subjects for this study are adults and musically untrained, it is
important to understand how they listen. In regard to the manner in which non-
musicians learn to listen, Madsen and Geringer (1990) indicated that musicians attend to
listening significantly differently than non-musicians. Musicians spend most of their
time attending to melody first, followed by rhythm, dynamics, and timbre, respectively.
Non-musicians spend the majority of their time focusing on dynamics, followed by
melody, timbre, and rhythm, respectively. Blocher (1990), however, found no significant
differences in the ability of musicians and non-musicians to attend to errors in
articulation, rhythm, phrasing, intonation, dynamics or note accuracy. Geringer and
Madsen (1995), in a study where the subjects were asked to note the prominence of
musical elements after they listened to a musical excerpt, found that musicians and non-
musicians had different listening patterns. Musicians listed timbre more frequently than
non-musicians did. A study by Wolpert (2000) showed that non-musicians did not hear
the difference in key or dissonance as well as musicians did. In that study, musicians
heard the difference in key 100% of the time whereas non-musicians heard it only 40% of
the time. Deutsch (1982) suggests that in recognizing a segment of music, we employ
global as well as specific cues, such as overall pitch range and distribution of interval
sizes, among others, so that melodies can be identified by their specific cues as a whole.
Earlier research results by Madsen (1987) and, Madsen and Wolf, (1979) suggest that
people attend to whatever they believe they should be attending to. This implies that non-
musicians can be directed to listen for specific ideas in sound.
Another consideration is the manner in which adults learn. The adult learner has
specific needs and expectations which differ greatly from elementary and secondary
school students. Lindeman introduced the pragmatic nature of adult learning in 1926
when he wrote that the approach to adult education should be focused on the situation
rather than the subject. Tough (1979) found that many adults start a learning project
because they anticipate using knowledge in a concrete way, strong motivation to gain and
retain knowledge that will produce some lasting change. Similarly, Scheckley (1983)
18
found that immediate knowledge was the reason most adults begin learning projects.
Literature on adult teaching strategies suggests the development of short, intensive
learning experiences with direct applications to the adult students’ lives (Wratcher and
Jones, 1988; Pomerance, 1991). Thus, lessons for adults should be interactive, well
managed with regard to time, and most of all, practical with regard to future use. This
would suggest that the present study should attend to adult’s learning needs. The lessons
should be relevant, intensive, and short in nature.
Motivation for Listening
Wlodkoski’s (1985) Time Continuum Model of Motivation focuses on the
internal motivation of adults. Wlodkowski defines this as “a condition expressed by the
individual as an internal force that leads the person to move in the direction of the goal”
(p. 17). He believes that the motivation to learn is internal and depends on the students’
needs and expectations at a given time in the learning process. This model has been
applied successfully in research by Mullee (1996) and would serve well as a basis for the
present study.
Figure 2.3 Wlodkoski’s Time Continuum Model for Motivation
19
The model consists of three time frames in the learning process: beginning, duration and
end. For each time frame there are two major factors of motivation present. The
beginning motivation is mostly effected by the learners’ attitudes towards the general
learning environment, instructor subject matter, self, and the basic needs of the learner at
the onset of learning. During the learning process, motivation is influenced by the
stimulation and interest of the learner and the affective experience that learning provides.
At the end, the learner is able to apply new skills and become aware of new knowledge,
thereby feeling competent.
At the beginning of the process, the instructor needs to create a positive,
comfortable learning atmosphere to establish positive learner attitudes. Wodolkski goes
on to write that the beginning time frame is a critical period in determining the way
learners respond to and feel about what they are experiencing. At the same time that
attitude is being established and supported, the instructor should also attempt to maintain
learner attention and build learner interests. To insure a positive affective experience and
emotional climate, learners need from the instructor encouragement and assistance that
integrate their emotions within the learning process, methods and strategies that
emphasizes cooperation and maximize learner involvement, and sharing that contributes
to a supportive environment. Finally, the instructor should acknowledge the positive
changes that learning has produced and affirm and continue motivation for application
and future learning.
20
Part Two
On Visual Stimuli
Visual Stimuli and Instruction
Research in learning style shows that individuals utilize different types of stimuli,
auditory, visual, tactile and kinesthetic, to enhance learning. One of those stimuli is
visual, or using the sense of sight. Visual learning represents a particular form of human
achievement, one that includes the ability to notice what is visually subtle and use it in
ways that are personally meaningful (Eisner, E. 1998). Eisner also goes on to state that
visual learning pertains not only to our capacity to construe meaning from the visual
forms around us but to our capacity to create connections of visual stimuli with other
forms of stimuli including auditory stimuli. Apart from listening to auditory stimuli, in
the form of lecture and verbal instruction, visual stimuli are used extensively in
instruction in the form of charts, pictures, diagrams and video imaging. The visual
stimulus is then processed into a meaningful representation for the individual enhancing
what he or she is learning. Research indicates that the amount of information that can
be processed using both auditory and visual channels may be considerably larger than
information processed using a single channel (Kalyuga, Chandler, and Sweller, 2000).
Studies, including those by Anglin (1986), Ritchey (1982) and Yang & Weidman (1993),
demonstrate that visual learning can positively affect cognitive processes such as recall
and problem solving. The appropriate use of visuals can enhance recall and
understanding of material, increase motivation, and promote critical thinking (Blatnik,
1998; Levie and Lentz, 1982; Levie 1987; and Peeck 1987). The manner in which
information is presented visually can make a significant difference, especially for
students who have difficulty in one or more perception areas (Dickey, J.P. & Hendricks,
R.C 1991). It is noteworthy that the visual does not have to directly explain a concept in
order to provide a link for the memory process. In Imagery and Related Mnemonic
Devices by McDaniel and Pressley (1987), the authors point out numerous examples of
how visual stimuli do not have to be a complete one-to-one relationship with the
particular concept or event to be effective in creating a memory link.
21
Since the use of visual stimuli can affect memory and thus learning, the use of
visual stimuli should be made to enhance the music learning as well. The use of visual-
spatial stimuli to reinforce auditory discrimination has been incorporated in some popular
approaches to music teaching such as the Orff or Kodaly approaches. These approaches
make use of graphic representation of sound prior to learning musical notation and they
meet with strong endorsements from theorists and practitioners alike (Boardman, E.,
2001; Boardman & Andress, B., 1981, and Nye R.E. & Nye V.T., 1977). In fact,
research has been conducted using visual stimuli and music to indicate positive
relationships. Results of a study by Forsythe, J.C. & Kelly, M.M. (1989) suggest that the
use of visual cues paired with melodies is an effective aid to aural discrimination among
fourth grade subjects. In this study, 30 brief melodies were paired with visual cues in the
form of hand cues. There was a significant difference in identifying melodies between
those students who received visual cues and those students who did not. Other studies
have also shown positive results pairing visual and aural modalities. Olson (1978, 1981)
conducted several studies concerned with the perception of melodic contour in visual and
aural modes. Subjects were asked to determine whether visual and aural stimuli
presented matched and although research proved not to be significant it did suggest that
aural and visual stimuli could be paired temporally. Hair (1993) showed that children
were able to articulate descriptions of music when they were allowed to use drawn
representations or symbols and icons. Hair (1995) also showed that color was effective
when asking both adults and children to associate color to mood and a musical
composition.
While visual art is expressed in space and music is expressed through time, and
each is unique as an art form, music and art do have common linkage. Both evoke
human feelings and both rely on the concept of unity and contrast for expression.
Specifically, it is the structure that makes it possible to correlate art and musical stimuli.
Goldberg, and Schrack, (1986) stated that the correlation of musical and visual structures
is a theoretical discipline to be worked out creatively, similar to counterpoint or common
practice harmony. It involves studying the basic concepts common to both visual and
musical arts such as line, texture, rhythm and color and analyzing works of artists like
Kupka, Kandinsky and Klee who have developed ideas of correlation. Limbert and
22
Polzella (1998) showed that listening to matching music while viewing paintings
apparently intensified the listening experience. Haack (1970) showed that the use of
visual exemplars was found to enhance the development of the desired musical concepts
significantly and to bring about a definite improvement in related art viewing skills.
Stravinsky and Scriabin possessed skills of color hearing and their compositions express
richness in timbre. Scriabin assigned each pitch a direct color through chordal
complexities and according to some sources, he deduced the full cycle from his
spontaneous recognition of C=red, D=yellow and F#= Blue (Shaw-Miller, 2002).
Composers are not the only artistic individuals to have chromosthesis. It is documented
that Kandinsky also had this trait and painted to express colors in sound (Maur, 1999).
Not only did Kandinsky have chromosthesis but he related art to music in a direct way.
He wrote, “Color is the keyboard, the eye is the hammer, the soul the piano. The artist is
the hand that purposefully sets the vibrating by means of this or that key” (Lindsay &
Vergo, 1982).
Studies using music to enhance art skills have also been conducted. Limbert M.
and Polzella (1998) clearly showed that the music affects the artistically naive listeners
while viewing representational and abstract paintings in their perceptions of the paintings.
All of the above information gives validity to using visual stimuli in music instruction.
This research attempts to study the effects of paintings on a subject’s ability to listen and
recall musical elements.
Historical Connection Between Visual Art and Music
Music and art have been intertwined and compared since early times. In the early
17
th
century, in his Critical Reflections on Poetry and Painting (1715), the Abbe Dubos
claimed that “Just as paintings represent the forms and colors of nature, so does music
represent the tones, the accents, the sighs, the modulations of the voice, in short all of the
sounds through which nature itself expresses the feelings and passions…”. Another early
analogy between painting and music appeared in 1762 in Giovanni Berllori’s Lives of
Modern Painters, Sculptors and Architects. Describing Lanfranco’s ceiling decoration at
St. Andrea dell Valle in Rome, Bellori wrote, “ This painting has been rightly compared
to full-bodied music, in which all the tones come together to form a harmony.” In 1849,
23
Thomas Purdie published a book entitled Form and Sound: Can their beauty be
dependent on the same physical laws? Purdie sought to demonstrate that visual artistic
beauty was universally based on mathematical ratios, manifested in music as the
harmonic rations of vibrating strings (Scheuller, H. M. 1953). From this comparison and
relationship, composers and painters alike have been influenced and created art based on
the other art form. Sometimes the connections can be loose and vague, other times more
intertwined.
Visual artists in the 1800’s explored expressing specific musical elements or
musical form. By the end of the nineteenth century, music and art were trading key ideas
back and forth. In 1853, John Ruskin stated, “We are to remember that the arrangement
of colors and lines is an art analogous to the composition of music.” (Kagan, 1986). A
prime instance is Claude Debussy’s seemingly impressionistic technique of “stippled
notes”. His suppression of the principle musical development through time in favor of
juxtaposed fields of contrasting tone color was also used in visual arts of the same time
period. By the 1890’s, musical elements in paintings often went hand in hand with an
allegorical symbolism. In Gustav Klimt’s Music, a sphinx stands for the infallible nature
of music (Maur, K. 1999). Klimt also translated the hymn from Beethoven’s 9
th
Symphony “Fruede Schoner Gotterfunken Diesen Kuss der gazen Welt” into visual
allegory (Willsdon, 1996). Another example of allegory and imagery used to cross the
visual and the music world is the work of Mendelssohn. Mendelsshon used visual
imagery to express his music, including Hebrides and his Symphony Number 2, the
Italian (Grey, T. 1997).
In addition to allegorical and imagery representation, composers and painters
alike have frequently gleaned ideas from or borrowed from procedures in sibling arts.
The reciprocal relationship runs like a thread throughout the 19
th
and 20
th
century. Using
similar symbolic information is one example of this. Leitmotif has the status of symbol,
which is often subjected to patternization, variation, development or metamorphoses.
With the concept of patterns as a basis, it is possible to employ a visual pattern that is
symbolic of meaning that works in the same manner as the leitmotif (Goldberg &
Schrack, 1986). The romantics envisaged breaking down the barriers between the
various genres to create a Gesamtkunstwerk, a total, comprehensive, or what today would
24
be termed a multimedia or interdisciplinary work of art. In this, music was granted a
leading role (Maur, K. 1999). Phillip Otto Runge saw in music the common primordial
source of all the arts and a guarantor of beauty. He discovered the possibility of a
figurative painting, his Lesson of the Nightingale, on the basis of the fugal principle of
imitation.
Art in Music Education
Art is found in education, both within and outside the music classroom. Outside
the music classroom art has been used in courses such as English and History. Erickson
(1995) found that students in history classes were able to incorporate knowledge of
individual artists and develop a historical perspective as they looked at artworks. In each
of these examples, art is used to enhance the learning in other disciplines either as
allegorical, symbolic or direct representation. As in music, art is used in history texts to
reinforce time periods as well as to depict historical scenes and events.
Music educators often advocate a multi-sensory approach to learning,
particularly as a way of accommodating individual differences among learners. Using
multi-sensory approach brings excitement in learning of all subjects for both teachers as
well as students (Wang & Sogin, 1998,1998,1991). Music curriculum text books use art
to reinforce musical elements, addressing the needs of visual learners. Examples of
integration of art in music lessons are found in the most recent and popular texts
including Making Music (2002), Share the Music (1995) and Music Connection (1995).
In all three of these text series, art works are used to reinforce music concepts including
timbre, theme and variation, and musical imagery. At the secondary and collegiate level,
music history texts such as History of Western Music by K M. Stolba use art works to
emphasize historical period and to reinforce the relationship between the art and music of
a given time period.
Application of Gestalt Principles in Art and Music
Gestalt theory is a broadly interdisciplinary general theory which provides a
framework for a wide variety of psychological phenomena, processes, and applications.
Human beings are viewed as open systems in active interaction with their environment.
25
It is especially suited for the understanding of order and structure in psychological events
and has its origins in some orientations of Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, Ernst Mach, and
particularly of Christian von Ehrenfels and the research work of Max Wertheimer,
Wolfgang Köhler, Kurt Koffka, and Kurt Lewin (Gordon 1989). According to Keith
Swanick, Gestalt psychology is “the organization of sensory information into meaningful
wholes based on prior experience” (1988). The sensory information is then grouped by
one of four ways: Proximity, Similarity, Common Direction or Simplicity. These four
make up the following four laws: 1.) The law of proximity: Elements are grouped
according to nearness in space or time; 2.) The law of similarity: Objects or events are
grouped with “same” attributes, such as timbre, color or shape; 3.) The law of common
direction: Elements are grouped according to their extrapolated completion; 4.) The law
of simplicity: Information is grouped with a preference for smoothness, symmetry and
regularity (Radocy, R & Boyle, J., 1988). The principles of Gestalt provide the basis for
learners to discover the underlying nature of a topic or problem (i.e., the relationship
among the elements). Gaps, incongruities or disturbances are an important stimulus for
learning and instruction should be based upon the laws of organization: proximity,
closure, similarity and simplicity.
These four laws and principles of Gestalt are often applied in the visual realm or
to separate theories of visual perception. The visual realm in and out of art has relied on
the principles of Gestalt to support its work. Visual perception is the bedrock on which
many ideas inevitably build their foundation (Kepes, G. 1965). The use of imagery and
visual thinking processes are primary ways of exploring, expressing and communicating
the known and imagined properties of a system, theory or general phenomenon
(Arnheim, 1954, 1974). Visual ways of thinking and learning move across, between and
through disciplinary commitments. This connection through vision creates a kind of
convergence that defines contemporary views of interdisciplinary areas. Vision is a
natural connecting force that can reestablish relationships that have been obscured by
arbitrary divisions (Klein, 1990). The Gestalt principles of similarity, continuity,
proximity and closure are the primary factors and forces that create and emphasize visual
units, groupings and organized wholes within a given perceptual setting. The degree of
visual order or disorder perceived within a setting is dependent to a large degree on the
26
recognition, interpretation and communication of these unifying principles (Wenger,
1997). Gestalt is part of a language of vision present in the simplest forms of mark
making as well as the complex configurations found in a work of art. In art, a unified
entity or whole can be a singular composition or individual graphic elements that make
up the totality of a creative work.
Since both art and music can be analyzed by studying the underlying structure, the
Gestalt principles can be applied to both visual and auditory stimuli. Musicians have not
researched extensively the use of the principles of Gestalt with auditory stimuli, however
some studies do exist. As early as 1890, Ehrenfels introduced the concept of
Gestaltqualitat. This concept was explained using a musical example. “When we hear a
tune, the experience of the tune itself (the gestaltqualitat) is something more than the
aggregate of the notes. It is not reducible to individual notes and is not an adding
together of simple sensations. For example, the last three notes of ‘God Save the Queen’
are the same as the first three notes of ‘Three Blind Mice’.” (Gordon, 1989, p.55). Even
Leonard Meyer, who subscribes to the theory of an emotional response to music, argues
that the work of Gestalt Psychologists has shown beyond a doubt that understanding is
not a matter of perceiving single stimuli, or simple sound combinations in isolation, but is
rather a matter of grouping stimuli into patterns and relating these patterns to one another.
(1956, p. 6). Meyer goes on to explain that the mind in its selection and organization of
discrete stimuli into figures and groupings appears to obey certain general laws, including
the Law of Good Continuation. He points out that the general laws that the mind follows
to group items operates within a socio-cultural context. It can be inferred from this that
listeners use this Gestalt Law to listen to music that has characteristics that are
identifiable within a particular socio-cultural context. This would help to support why
people listen for the familiar before moving on to the unfamiliar.
In support of Contour, Dowling (1994) explains the role of Gestalt Principles in
contour. He states that a melody is very much an integrated whole, a Gestalt. He points
out that the tonal context affects memory for contour and that contour interacts with both
tonality and rhythm in perception and memory. In his Auditory Scene Analysis,
Bregman (1990) describes a range of perceptual processes that enable us to construct an
auditory picture of the environment and form sensory data much the way Dowling
27
suggests the listener deals with contour. Bregman identified two types of processes: a.)
primitive, automatic process and b.) Schema driven or learnt processes. The cues used
by the primitive processes to group sound events together to construct patterns are akin to
the Gestalt Laws of grouping. It has been shown that Gestalt Laws operate in the
perception of visual arrays and it can be said that it is true in of music also. In the case of
vision, elements that are close together in space are more likely to belong to the same
objects than are elements that are spaced further apart.
The same line of reasoning holds for elements that are similar rather than those
that are dissimilar (Deutsch, D. 1999). In the case of hearing, similar sounds are likely to
have originated from a common source and dissimilar sounds from different sources. A
sequence that changes smoothly in frequency is likely to have originated from a single
source, whereas an abrupt frequency transition may reflect the presence of a new source.
Components of a complex spectrum that arise in synchrony are likely to have emanated
from the same source, and the sudden addition of a new component may signal the
emergence of a new source (Deutsch, D. 1999 pp. 300-301). A sequence of musical
tones tends to be heard as groupings of organized metrical, rhythmic, melodic and
harmonic units. Smaller units are joined together to form larger units in an embedded,
hierarchical fashion. The tones are then grouped together according to function, and
other attributes. These groupings are then perceived as similar based on such things as
similarity in frequency, spatial location, or having temporally synchronous onsets or
offsets (Krumhansl, C. 1990). Royal and Fiske (2000) took these identified processes
and their relationship to Gestalt and suggested grouping boundaries along various
dimensions of sound based on the four Gestalt Laws. They suggest that boundaries
between groups are likely to be apparent where these laws are broken. Royal and Fiske
suggest that for each Gestalt Principle the musical concepts of pitch, time, timbre
loudness and space have a relationship. Pitch and proximity are apparent when there is a
change in register. Time has a relationship in similarity when there is a change in
articulation. The example in the table below uses pitch, time and duration, timbre,
loudness, and space to illustrate the boundaries and their association with each of the
gestalt laws.
28
Figure 2.4 Music and Association with Gestalt Laws.
Grouping boundaries along various dimensions of sound * contrary to:
*Proximity *Similarity *Good Continuation *Common Fate
Pitch change in register change in melodic contrary or
direction oblique
Time rests and long notes articulation change in pulse onsets and
offsets
Timbre change in timbre evolution of evolving
timbre over components
time of timbre
Loudness Changes in loudness unpredictable change differing rates/
and stress in loudness directions of
of loudness
Space spatially separate unpredictable moving in
Sources movement in space different
Directions/
Along diff.
Trajectories in
space
Koniari, Predazzer and Melen ( 2001) suggest that listeners are able to build a mental
representation of a piece but that mental representation does not keep all of the details of
the actual piece; instead listeners pick up and focus on specific cues, or cue abstraction.
(Referred to as cue extraction in Deliege, 1987, 1989; Deliege & El Ahmadi, 1990).
While listening to a piece of music, listeners pick up from the musical surface small
entities that contrast sufficiently to attract listeners’ attention. The cues provide temporal
landmarks: the passages based on a given cue are approximated and localized by the
listener in the course of the musical piece. Listeners are then assumed to be able to
reorder the different segments along a mental line resulting in a mental schema. Neisser
(1976, p. 54) states “A schema… is internal to the perceiver, modifiable by experience
and somehow specific to what is being perceived. The schema accepts information as it
becomes available at sensory surfaces and is changed by that information.” The cues also
29
constitute the basis of a categorization process. This categorization process then relies on
the categories of Gestalt to help listeners place information in a useable, retrievable place
within memory. The abstracted cues are the bases on which different structures of a
piece are compared to each other. All of this would suggest that the law of similarity can
be very effective given that listeners use cue abstraction to obtain musical information. If
students are encouraged to listen for and view examples of similarity during listening
lessons it is suggested that it will help during recall.
Memory and Listening
While the Gestalt Principles enable people’s perceptions of visual and auditory
stimuli, learning takes place only when the new information can be stored as memory in
the brain. The listener does not initially remember exactly what was heard but
remembers certain global features of overall pattern such as contour and key (Dowling,
1978; Dowling & Barrlet, 1981; Dewit and Crowder, Dowling Et el, 1995/1998;
Dowling, Tillman & Ayers 2002). Tulving (1983/1984) suggests a theory of episodic
memory where memory, while listening to music, stores traces of past events to recall
during a new listening experience. Both of these theories suggest that information that a
listener possesses on contour, rhythm and other musical elements will enable the listener
to remember salient features of the musical composition. Further, in this study, the
participant is asked to use inter-sensory memory systems. Research by Berlin (1976),
Goehr (1990), and Peeck (1987) suggests that music is an inter-sensory experience and
that auditory images in memory are often accompanied by kinesthetic and visual images.
This was further proven by Mitchell and Gallaher (2001) with studies of children, music
and memory of dance and visual image. Paivio (1986) suggests that memory involves
separate but interacting storage systems for verbal information and nonverbal images.
Pavio also suggests that verbal input alone is less likely to be stored in verbal and
nonverbal systems whereas visual information presented with verbal information is more
likely to be stored and retrieved. This information would indicate that the participants in
this study who are given more than one modality will be more likely to be successful in
remembering and recalling information than those participants who are only given one
modality.
30
An additional type of memory that is crucial for this study is recall or memory
over an amount of time. Recall is the ability to retain and reproduce information at a later
time. The amount of time between recall is the ability to retain and reproduce
information at a later time. The amount of time between obtaining information and when
it is recalled can be categorized under short or long term memory, depending on when it
is recalled. Short-term memory is also sometimes referred to as working memory.
Research suggests that within the brain, temporal oscillators provide the context that
serves as the basis for retrieval. Temporal oscillators are neural cell assemblies that fire
in regular cycles but with different oscillators cycling at different frequencies.
Oscillators therefore provide a rich signal that change continually over time (Mayberry,
et.al, 2002). Burgess and Hitch (1996) and Hitch et el. (1996) include temporal
oscillators as one of several features in models on short-term or working memory. In
these models, the current signal from a set of oscillators is associated with each item to be
remembered. Then, at recall, the oscillators are reset to the point they had occupied in
their cycles at the commencement of the recall. As the oscillators are replayed, their
changing signal then provides cues for the ability to recall (Mayberry, et al, 2002).
Memory is crucial for any listener. Without an ability to remember and recall the
listener is not capable of making choices about a presented piece of music, whether it be
the first time the listener hears the music or subsequent times.
31
Hypothesis
Based on the discussions above regarding the relationships of visual stimuli and strategies
used in listening, it is logical to assume that pointing out similarities in paintings may
assist elementary education majors to identify musical elements while listening to a
musical composition. For this research it is hypothesized that “Using paintings in music
listening lessons will enhance musical listening skills.”
© Jennifer Sue Shank, 2003
32
CHAPTER 3
METHODOLOGY
Introduction
Elementary education majors at The University of Kentucky served as subjects
for this study. The university is a nationally ranked institution with over 28,000
students. The University offers over 50 Education related degrees and certificate
programs, with 1,452 students enrolled in the College of Education at the undergraduate
level. The College of Education has a diverse population including African American,
Native American, Asian and Hispanic Students. This school was selected for this study
on the basis of available subjects and access to classes appropriate to the study. The
treatment was administered during the second semester of Teaching Methods in the
Elementary Grades. The treatment took place in a music education research classroom
on the campus of the university. The classroom was set up for various music activities
and music education activities including listening, performance and movement. The
experimental group received instruction as well as viewing visually projected paintings
during the listening lesson. The control group received only instruction while listening to
music. Both groups received treatment by the same instructor and pre-tested and post-
tested using the same test. The data was analyzed to find whether or not the group
receiving integrated art and music lesson scored higher at the post-test.
Selection of Subjects
Four of the five sections of MUS 261- Teaching Methods in Elementary Grades
were chosen for participation in this study. Students enrolled in all sections of the class
without prior knowledge of the study. The chosen sections were informed verbally and
in writing at the beginning of the semester that they would be involved in the study.
Students meeting the following criteria were candidates for this study: (a.) elementary
education or special education majors (b) completion of Music for Elementary Education
Majors I, or proof of competency of concepts taught in that class (c) completion of all
33
parts of the study and (d) no excessive absences during the pre-test and post-test as well
as the duration of the study.
The subjects ranged in age from 20 to 36, with varied musical skills. Subjects
were polled at the beginning of the class for previous musical experience and previous art
appreciation courses they may have taken. Subjects ranged from students with very little
experience other than the first method’s class, to accomplished musicians with ten or
more years of playing or singing experience. Subjects also ranged from students that
have taken multiple art appreciation or art education classes to those students who have
had no formal art education. The four classes met on Mondays, Wednesdays and
Fridays for 50-minute sessions between the hours of 8:00am to 1:00pm and were taught
by a team of two instructors. Students in each class were randomly placed into the
experimental group or the control group creating eight subgroups. These eight subgroups
received instruction from both instructors with each instructor teaching different topics.
Research Design
The design for this study was a Pre-test Post-test Experimental Design with the
random assignment of subjects to either the control or experimental group from four
intact class groups of approximately 25 students each. Ninety-three subjects completed
the study with 46 subject in the control group and 47 subjects in the experimental group.
Figure 3.1 below shows the design and procedure for the study.
34
Figure 3.1: Research Study Design Model
Pretest
N=100 ( MUS 261)
TMLST
Treatment 1
Experimental
N=47
Listening lesson with call chart and
paintings
Treatment 2
Control
N=46
Listening lesson with call
chart only
Post Test
N=93
TMLST
35
Instrumentation
Teacher Music Listening Skills Test (TMLST)
The Teacher Music Listening Skills Test was designed by the researcher and was
used for both the pre-test and post-test. The content of the test aligns with the course
materials studied by the students. The purpose of the pre-test was to establish that the
subjects had knowledge of musical elements and vocabulary including expressive
elements and form types. The pre-test would also indicate whether the subjects were
homogeneous with regard to listening ability. The Teacher Music Listening Skills Test
(TMLST) took approximately 25 minutes and was administered prior to the beginning of
the treatment and immediately after the treatment. The test consisted of two parts.
TMLST-A had ten questions. Six of the questions were matching items and four were
multiple choice items with a maximum score of ten. Part A measured basic knowledge
and vocabulary about musical expressive elements and form. TMLST Part B is the main
section of the test, and it measures the listening skills of the subjects needed to identify
the musical elements in the musical excerpts. (See Appendix A). Musical excerpts were
1:00 to 1:12 long. The TMLST-B had one fill in the blank and seven items that were
multiple-choice with a maximum possible score of 64.
To establish validity of the test, a panel of six music experts took the test
independently. The music experts consisted of music educators, graduate students in
music education, and professional musicians. They agreed that responses to the test were
indicative of music listening skills and musical knowledge required for listening. The
reliability was tested by the use of interjudge reliability. Fifty percent of both the pre-test
and post-test of the TMLST taken by subjects were judged by another researcher to
calculate the interjudge reliability. The test was developed, revised and piloted over a
period of weeks using other students with similar backgrounds prior to this study which
prompted several changes to improve the instrument and administrative aspect of the test.
(See appendix H for the pilot study.)
The post-test was identical to the pre-test with the exception that the order of the
presentation of the musical excerpts was changed. For each excerpt, in addition to items
36
for listening skills, two items of self-rating scales were added. Students were asked to
rate the Familiarity of the piece on a scale of 1-7 and they were also to rate Preference,
how well they liked the piece, also on a scale of 1-7.
37
Procedure
Permission for using subjects in this study was obtained through the university
Office of Human Research Studies during the semester prior to the study. Students were
also informed in writing and verbally at the beginning of the semester that class work
would be used in an upcoming research study during the course of the semester. No
indication was made of exact assignments or units during the course of the semester. (See
Appendix C and D)
The pre-test was administered during the third week of the spring semester. The
Teacher Music Listening Skills Test was administered on Monday, the week prior to the
treatment. The test took 25 minutes to administer and was administered in the music
education resource room where all MUS 261 classes meet regularly on a Monday,
Wednesday, Friday schedule. The students took the pre-test at their regularly scheduled
class time with no differentiation between the control and treatment group. The test
included a practice excerpt so students could familiarize themselves with the test
procedure prior to taking the test items. Each student was given a copy of the
instructions and questions and a packet of answer sheets. Directions for the listening test
were read aloud and students were given a practice excerpt. The excerpt was played once
and students were to answer questions in Part B through question number seven.
Questions one through seven involved listening to general characteristics of the piece
such as instruments texture and rhythm. Then, for question number eight, a shorter
version of the same excerpt of eight to twelve seconds was played to highlight only the
main melody and the students were to select one of the given contours that matched the
melody. (See appendix A) After the practice excerpt and students were given a chance
to ask questions, the test was administered without pause. Students were asked to
complete, and given time to complete, Part A before moving on to the listening portion of
Part B. At the end of the eighth excerpt students were asked to double check that their
name and class section was on the answer sheet and then both the answer packet and the
question sheets were collected.
38
The class period after the tests were administered the students were randomly
assigned to either the control or experimental groups and students were to attend 25
minutes of the 50 minute class with the researcher and 25 minutes of the 50 minute class
with the second instructor who was presenting different material. Students were not
informed of the differences in the split classes, just that they were split and depending on
the group attended class with the researcher first or class with the other instructor first.
The preparation included planned instructional content and instructional
environment. In order to control for extraneous variables, the researcher delivered all of
the instruction for this study. Prior to the experimental treatment all subjects reviewed
music concepts and elements for listening. For the treatment, both the experimental and
control groups met in a “smart” classroom during the study. This classroom was
equipped with projection equipment and a computer in addition to the standard
equipment found in a classroom. The visual presentation of the paintings for the sections
receiving the treatment was projected onto a screen in the classroom using a projector
attached to the classroom computer and a PowerPoint slide presentation. The musical
examples for both the treatment and the control group were played from a single CD,
compiled and burned by the researcher, with each excerpt lasting between two and three
minutes. Salient information was written on a dry erase board, such as composer, title of
composition, contour shape, texture and instrumentation. Both the treatment and the
control groups met three times a week on a Monday, Wednesday, Friday schedule.
Classes met in the morning or early afternoon, depending on the class. No classes met
after 1:00pm in the afternoon.
A final preparatory measure concerned the possible cancellation of class due to
severe inclement weather. In that event, class would be cancelled for the duration of the
severe inclement weather and class would resume where it ended at the conclusion of the
severe weather. The class schedule would be modified to accommodate all of the
scheduled lessons for the treatment of the study. No lesson would be omitted from the
study.
39
Treatment
The treatment phase consisted of six teaching sessions within a span of two
weeks. The sessions were divided as follows: three sessions in week one and three
sessions in week two, following a Monday, Wednesday, Friday schedule. Each session
lasted a total of 25 minutes. Each session for both the treatment and control groups was
taught by the researcher. Each session was scripted (See Appendix E) so that both the
treatment and control groups were to receive identical instruction, with the exception of
the addition of the visual stimuli in the form of paintings projected in front of the class
via a PowerPoint presentation for the experimental group. The scripted lessons were
strictly adhered to by the researcher to ensure that lesson contents and delivery were
identical for each treatment session.
The same instructional strategy was followed in each session. Class began with
roll call followed by a brief introduction of the listening piece for that particular day. The
introduction included information regarding title, composer, and a general historical
background. Students listened to the musical excerpt a total of three times throughout
each session. The excerpt was first played after the general information was presented to
the class. Students were asked to just listen to the piece. After students listened to the
excerpt once, salient features were discussed such as instrumentation, contour, texture
and beat structure. The second time the piece was played students were instructed to
listen for the specific items discussed. After the second playing, students in the treatment
group that received paintings were shown projected images of the appropriate painting.
The salient features that most related to the music were pointed out and the music was
played a third and final time while the image remained projected for the students. The
control group heard the piece a third time without the presence of the paintings. There
were six musical examples all together, all instrumental, and all written for orchestra,
chamber orchestra or string quartet. Paintings were chosen based on several criteria
including the painting’s direct relationship to the piece of music or composer, musical
elements reflected in a visual way and the measured element of similarity apparent in
40
each work, and opinions of expert artists. In the treatment group, paintings were used as
a tool for analogy to stress what was illustrated in the lecture and on the dry erase board.
Music and Art Used in this Study
The music and art used in this study were chosen after careful consideration. The
musical pieces chosen for this study all had to meet certain strict criteria in order to
eliminate possible unforeseen musical effects. All pieces had to be instrumental and
performed by either full orchestra or string quartet. Each piece had to be usable when
two to three minute portions of excerpts were extracted, and still contain identifiable
melodies, harmonies and form structure that remained consistent throughout the piece.
Each musical piece had to have musical characteristics that were easily recognizable,
including clear instrumentation, texture and a clear melodic contour. Only musical
pieces from eras ranging from the baroque to early 20
th
century were considered. Pieces
associated with preconceived visual imagery or pieces that were traditionally labeled as
programmatic were not eligible. Pieces that had been used in mass marketing or
advertising were not eligible so as to prevent any preconceived association with imagery
or a particular product. Finally, musical pieces selected for this study needed to embody
the Gestalt Laws of Similarity as laid out by Christian Von Ehrenfels and reinforced
musically by Royal and Fiske (2002).
Following the above guidelines the following pieces were used in this research:
The Soldier’s Tale by Igor Stravinsky measures 1-36, recorded by the Los Angeles
Chamber Orchestra and conducted by Gerard Schwarz in 1984. A Night in the Tropics
the andante movement by Louise M. Gottschalk meausures 1-56, recorded by The Utah
Symphony Orchestra and conducted by Maurice Abravanel in 1986. Symphony No 9,
fourth movement by Ludwig von Beethoven measures 13-36, recorded by The London
Classical Players and conducted by Roger Norrington in 1987. Symphony No 94 in G
Major Andante movement by Franz Joseph Haydn measures 1-48, recorded by The
London Philharmonic Orchestra and conducted by Sir George Solti in 1984.
Contrapunctus 2 by J.S. Bach the entire piece, recorded by The Juilliard String Quartet in
1992. Valse Triste by Jean Sibelius measures 9- 56, recorded by The Estonian National
Symphony Orchestra and conducted by Paavo Jarvi in 2002. All recordings were
41
digitally copied from compact discs onto a single compact disc using I-Tunes® software
by Apple.
Figure 3.2: Music Chosen for this study
Title Composer
The Soldier’ Tale Igor Stravinsky
A Night in the Tropics Louis M. Gottschalk
Symphony #94 F.J. Haydn
Symphony #9 Ludwig von Beethoven
Contrapunctus #2 J. S. Bach
Valse Triste Jean Sibelius
Paintings chosen for this study fell into 2 distinct categories: paintings that were
directly related to a musical selection in some way as indicated by previous literature and
paintings that had a relationship with a musical selection in a general or causal manner.
For example; Beethoven Frieze: Hymn of Joy (detail) 1902 by Gustav Klimt was painted
as a direct visual representation of Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony and specifically, the last
movement of the symphony. Sunday on the Island of La Grande Jatte 1884-1886, oil on
canvas by George Seurat has a casual relationship using the pointillism in the painting to
support the music of Maurice Gottschalk and his extensive use of staccato within A Night
in the Tropics. Paintings, regardless of category, were chosen based on a set of art
elements criteria that work well with the Gestalt Principle of Similarity and can be
applied. A panel of artists was asked to rank a pool of 15 paintings that fit these
parameters. The panel was comprised of four art professors and an art teacher. The
panel was asked to rank the paintings on a series of arts elements and how closely each
painting came to the Gestalt Principle of Similarity. The panel was asked to use a scale
of 1-10, to rate each painting with one being no similarity at all and 10 being extremely
similar. The elements that were focused on were color, form, shape, texture, balance,
pattern and rhythm. (See Appendix F and Appendix G). From the pool of 15 paintings,
eight paintings were considered to fit the criteria. The researcher chose six paintings that
scored the highest or received the most agreement by the art experts. From the eight
42
eligible paintings, six were chosen for this study. The paintings were: La Dance
1909/10, Oil canvas by Henri Matisse; Beethoven Frieze: Hymn of Joy (detail) 1902 by
Gustav Klimt; Fugue (Controlled Impression) 1914, oil on canvas by Wassily
Kandinsky; Fugue from the diptych Prelude and Fugue 1907, tempera on paper by
Mikalojus Konstantinas Ciurlionis; A Sunday on the Island of La Grande Jatte 1884-
1886, oil on canvas by George Seurat and Untitled 1932, oil on cardboard by Paul Klee
(See Appendix G).
Figure 3.3: Paintings Used in this Study
Painting Artist
La Dance Henri Matisse
Beethoven Frieze: Hymn of Joy Gustav Klimt
Fugue Wassily Kandinsky
Fugue M.K. Ciurlionis
Sunday on the Isle of La Grande Jatte G. Seurat
Untitled Paul Klee
Each painting and its related musical excerpt are listed below. The list of
paintings and music is displayed in two categories. Art and music with a direct
relationship for this study and art and music with an indirect relationship.
Figure 3.4 Art and Music Combined, Direct and Indirect Relationships
Directly Related Paintings and Music
Fugue by Ciurlionis Contrapunctus #2 by J. S. Bach
Beethoven Frieze by Klimt Symphony #9 by Beethoven
Fugue by Kandinsky Soldier’s Tale by Stravinsky
Indirectly Related Paintings and Music
Untitled by Paul Klee Valse Triste by Sibelius
Sunday on the Isle… by Seurat Night.. Tropics by Gottschalk
La Dance by Henri Matisse Symphony #94 byF.J. Haydn
43
The Teacher Music Listening Skills Test (TMLST) was administered as a post-
test during the class session that immediately followed the final treatment session. This
test was identical to the pre-test in content; however, the musical excerpts were presented
in a different order than in the pre-test. This was done in order to lessen the possibility, if
any, that students carry over what they remember from the pre-test instead of listening
anew. Using the same testing instrument for both pre-test and post-test allowed the
researcher to examine the amount of improvement in listening skills by the students and
minimizes extraneous variance due to different measuring tools.
The measure of reliability for the TMLST was obtained by having another
researcher score the tests independently. This independent judge was first trained to use
the grading criteria and the judge scored fifty percent of responses selected randomly
from all participants. The interjudge reliability was computed by Pearson r and was
found to be .89 (p<.001) for the pre-test and .93 (p<.001) for the post-test.
Apart from investigating whether the treatment had any effect on listening skills,
the researcher also wondered whether the treatment would make any difference in
students’ ability to recall musical information. A recall test was designed to collect data
for this secondary interest.
The second portion of the post-test, or the Recall Test, was administered 14 days
after the last day of treatment. The Recall Test was also designed by the researcher. The
test was designed to measure recall of salient musical features, visual imagery, as well as
title and composer information of pieces presented during the treatment sessions. The
Recall Test consisted of eight listening examples. Students were asked four questions
following each musical example. The musical examples chosen for the Recall Test
included four musical excerpts used during the treatment, two musical examples used as
detractors for the pre-test and post-test, and two musical excerpts that were completely
new. (See Appendix B)
The musical excerpts were played once each and students were given time to
answer the questions before the next excerpt was played. The measure of reliability for
the Recall Test was also obtained by having the same outside researcher score the tests
independently. The judge was trained to use the grading criteria and the judge scored
44
fifty percent of the responses selected randomly. The interjudge reliability was computed
by Pearson r and was found to be .86 (p>.001).
The procedure for this research was carried out with little problem.
Students adapted quickly and were very cooperative during the split classes. Students
had three minutes travel time between classrooms during the treatment and transition.
The use of equipment, recordings and the PowerPoint presentation also had no glitches or
problems to speak of. The overall time structure of the project was slightly affected by
inclement weather and the canceling of classes campus wide for a number of days. Due
to the campus closing and the inclement weather, the study was pushed back by one week
but no treatment sessions were changed or omitted because of this situation. The
research schedule was finished as planned.
There was some mortality in the study due to a variety of factors. At the onset of
the study 102 students took the pre-test. Ninety-three students completed the study in its
entirety. Students were lost during the study for the following reasons: Four students did
not attend at least five of the six research sessions or take the pre-test. Three students did
not take the post-test and could not schedule a make up time for the post test and one
student withdrew from the class during the study.
Other observations during the treatment sessions include the keeping of a journal
by the researcher. During the treatment portion of this study the researcher kept an
anecdotal journal of qualitative observations. Some observations indicative of the journal
included how students reacted during the viewing of the paintings in the case of the
experimental group and how students reacted to musical selections. A second
observation was obtaining demographic information about each of the study participants.
Since all of the students were enrolled in the same class, students were asked to
voluntarily fill out an information sheet with demographic information in it including
age, type of education prior to college, years of musical experience and what type of
musical experience if any it was. Students filled this sheet out the first day of classes in
the spring semester. The data was collected according to plan with good test reliability
and the statistics obtained from the pre-test and post-test are ready to be analyzed.
© Jennifer Sue Shank, 2003
45
CHAPTER 4
RESULTS
This study used a Pre-test Post-test Experimental design to measure the effects of
using paintings in the form of visually projected images to enhance music listening skills
of college education majors. The independent variable was the use of visual stimuli at
two levels, with or without the visually projected image of paintings. The dependent
variable was listening skills as indicated by the scores of the TMLST. The research
hypothesis “Using paintings in music listening lessons will enhance musical listening
skills,” was converted to the null hypothesis in order to test for statistical significance.
The null hypothesis for this research is, “Using paintings in music listening lessons has
no effect on musical listening skills.” The level of significance was set at .01. This
chapter will present preliminary descriptive statistics and the results of the statistical test
for significance for the hypothesis and related data.
The subjects’ demographic information is listed in Table 4.1. The subjects for
this study had a mean age of 21.3 years (sd=2.99). Ninety-seven percent of the students
were female and all were elementary music education majors at a major southern
university. The students had an average musical experience of 4.6 years with the
experience being evenly distributed between instrumental and choral music experience.
Musical experience included participating in ensembles at or above the high school level
or private lessons on an instrument or voice.
Table 4.1: Participant Demographic Information
Mean Age 21.3 years
Gender 97% female
3% male
Mean Musical Experience 4.6 years
_______________________________________
46
Results Related to Listening Skills
The testing instrument for this study was the Teacher Music Listening Skills Test
(TMLST) (see page 35). The test had a combined pre-test and post-test interjudge
reliability of .91 (p<.001). Table 4.2 shows the means and standard deviation on the Pre-
test for each of the eight subgroups created from the four sections used in the research.
Subgroup 1 had an n=12, a mean of 35.83 and an sd=6.25. Subgroup 2 had an n=12 with
a mean of 34.16 and an sd=6.17. Subgroup 3 had an n=8, a mean of 31.25 and an
sd=8.08. Subgroup 3 had the lowest number of subjects and the lowest mean score of the
eight subgroups because of absences within the study. As mentioned previously in this
study, several participants were not able to take the Pre-test or complete the study. Three
of these individuals came from Subgroup 3 and this is reflected in a lower mean score for
this subgroup. Subgroup 4 had an n=11, a mean of 32.27 and an sd=8.10. Subgroup 5
had an n=13, a mean of 36.69 and an sd=5.73. Subgroup 6 had an n=13, a mean of 37.90
and an sd=5.30. Subgroup 7 had an n=13, a mean of 34.76 and an sd=6.41. Subgroup 8
had an n=13, a mean of 35.30 and an sd=5.79. The pooled standard deviation was 6.45.
Table 4.2 Table of Means for TMLST Pre-test for Subgroups 1-8
(Max=78)
__________________________________________
Section Subgroup N Mean St. Dev
1 1 12 35.83 6.25
1 2 12 34.16 6.17
2 3 8 31.25 8.08
2 4 11 32.27 8.10
3 5 13 36.69 5.73
3 6 13 37.90 5.30
4 7 13 34.76 6.41
4 8 13 35.30 5.79
__________________________________________
47
In order to compare the TMLST pre-test scores for different subgroups, an
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used. No statistical significance was found due to
splitting groups into subgroups.
Table 4.3 shows that creating eight subgroups did not yield any significant
difference on pre-test listening scores (F=1.18, df=7, 85). This means that these
subgroups were considered equal before the experimental treatment.
Table 4.3: ANOVA Summary of TMLST for Pre-test Subgroups 1-8
_______________________________________________
Source DF SS MS F Prob
Group 7 343.0 49.0 1.18 ns
Error 85 3535.8 41.6
Total 92 1878.7
________________________________________________
Table 4.4 shows the means and standard deviations for the pre-test scores of
TMLST of the original four classes. Section One had an n=23, a mean of 35.13 and
sd=6.24. Section Two had an n=20, a mean of 31.85 and sd=7.67. Section Three had an
n= 24, a mean of 37.25 and sd=5.45. Section Four had an n=26, a mean of 35.03 and
sd=5.99. The pooled standard deviation was 6.32. Table 4.5 shows that ANOVA results
for the four sections. There is no statistical significance among the four sections (F=2.67,
df=3, 89).
Table 4.4 Table of Means for TMLST Pre-test for Classes 1-4
________________________________________________
Section Subgroup N Mean St. Dev
1 1&2 23 35.13 6.24
2 3&4 20 31.85 7.67
3 5&6 24 37.25 5.45
4 7&8 26 35.03 5.99
________________________________________________
48
Table 4.5: ANOVA Summary of TMLST for Class Sections1-4
___________________________________________
Source DF SS MS F Prob
Group 3 320.1 106.7 2.67 ns
Error 89 3558.8 40.0
Total 92 3878.7
___________________________________________
Since no difference in the TMLST was found among the four class sections or the eight
subgroups, one can assume that the participants were homogeneous in listening skills
before the treatment. Further analysis would focus the two groups as designated control
and experimental groups. The experimental group was comprised of subgroups 1, 3, 5
and 7 and the control group was comprised of subgroups 2, 4, 6 and 8. The mean Pre-test
score of the control group and experimental group were 34.97 and 34.91 respectively.
Table 4.6, below shows the ANOVA summary for control and treatment groups. The
data shows that the two groups are very close to identical, with an f- value close to zero
of 0.00, df=1,91 for the TMLST Pre-test.
Table 4.6: ANOVA Summary for Pre-test
_________________________________________
Source DF SS MS F Prob
Group 1 0.1 0.1 0.00 ns
Error 91 3878.6 42.6
Total 92 3878.7
_________________________________________
The descriptive statistics for the TMLST Part A and Part B are listed below in table 4.7
and 4.8. The maximum score for the TMLST is 74 points. The maximum score for part
A was 10 points. For part A, the control group scored a mean of 6.02 (sd=1.74) on the
49
pre-test and 6.43 (sd=4.33) on the post test. The experimental group scored 6.02
(sd=1.72) on the pre-test and 6.80 (sd=4.33) on the post-test. For part B of the TMLST,
pre-test the control group scored an average of 28.96 (sd=5.64) and the experimental
group scored an average of 28.89 (sd=5.60). Both groups improved from the pre-test to
the post-test. The control group scored an average of 29.65 (sd=4.62) on the post-test and
the experimental group scored an average score of 40.00 (sd=5.74).
Table 4.7: Mean Values of TMLST Part A (Max. 10 points)
_________________________________________
Pre-test Post-test
_________________________________________
Control Group 6.02 6.43
N=46 (sd=1.74) (sd=4.33)
Experimental Group 6.02 6.80
N= 47 (sd=1.72) (sd=4.33)
__________________________________________
Table 4.8: Mean Values of TMLST Part B (Max. 64 points)
___________________________________________
Pre-test Post-test
___________________________________________
Control Group 28.96 29.65
N=46 (sd=5.64) (sd=4.62)
Experimental Group 28.89 40.00
N= 47 (sd=5.60) (sd=5.79)
__________________________________________
50
As it was mentioned in Chapter Three, Part A of the TMLST measured the basic
knowledge and terminology involved in music listening while Part B contained items
specifically designed for measuring skills listening to recorded music excerpts. The
composite scores were used to compare the performance of the control group and the
experimental group. The Paired t-test was used to find out if each group improved
significantly from the Pre-test to the Post-test.
Table 4.9 TMLST Means Showing Improvement from Pre-test to Post-test
___________________Pre-test Post-test t-value
Control Group 34.97 35.84 ns
N=46 (sd=6.55) (sd=5.81)
________________________________________________
Experimental Group 34.91 47.26 10.11*
N= 47 (sd=6.50) (sd=6.08)
________________________________________________
*p<.001
Table 4.10 shows the results of ANOVA for TMLST Post-test means of the experimental
and control groups. The numbers shows that there is a significant difference with the
Post-test part B for the control and experimental groups with an f value of 87.97, df=1,
91, p<.001.
Table 4.10: ANOVA Summary of TMLST Post-test for the Experimental and Control
Groups
_______________________________________________
Source DF SS MS F Prob
Group 1 3116.1 3116.1 87.97 .001
Error 91 3223.4 35.4
Total 92 6339.6
________________________________________________
51
Scores of the TMLST for students in both the control group and the treatment
group did show an increase in value. However, the control group did not have a
statistically significant gain from the pre-test scores to post-test scores. As shown in
Table 4.9 the control group had a Pre-test mean of 34.97 (sd=6.55) and a Post-test mean
of 35.84 (sd=5.81), with a t-value of .35 which is not significant. The Experimental
group, however, did show a statistically significant improvement. The Pre-test score was
34.91 (sd=6.50) and a post-test score of 47.26 (sd=6.08) with a t value of 10.11, p<.001.
See Table 4.9.
Table 4.11 shows the means and standard deviation for TMLST Post-test for
Subgroups 1-8. The mean scores are much higher for those subgroups that were part of
the experimental group. Subgroups 1, 3, 5, and 7 were part of the experimental group.
Table 4.11 Table of Means for TMLST Post-test for Subgroups 1-8
(Max=78)
__________________________________________
Section Subgroup N Mean St. Dev
1 1 12 50.00 7.28
1 2 12 36.09 4.45
2 3 8 48.33 4.61
2 4 11 35.91 6.86
3 5 13 46.85 5.80
3 6 13 36.91 3.00
4 7 13 45.00 6.18
4 8 13 35.01 4.71
______________________________________________
52
Table 4:12 TMLST Means Showing Improvement From Pre-test to Post-test by
Subgroups 1-8.
___________________Pre-test Post-test
Exp.Sub Group 1 35.83 50.00
N=12 (sd=6.25) (sd=7.28)
Exp. Sub Group 3 31.25 48.33
N=8 (sd=8.08) (sd=4.61)
Exp. Sub Group 5 36.69 46.85
N=13 (sd=5.73) (sd=5.80)
Exp. Sub Group 7 34.76 45.00
(sd=6.41) (sd=6.18)
________________________________________________
Cont. Sub Group 2 34.16 36.09
N=12 (sd=6.17) (sd=4.45)
Cont. Sub Group 4 32.27 35.91
N=11 (sd=8.10) (sd=6.86)
Cont. Sub Group 6 37.90 36.91
N=13 (sd=5.30) (sd=3.00)
Cont. Sub Group 8 35.30 35.01
(sd=5.79) (sd=4.71)
________________________________________________
Table 4.12 shows the improvement by students in the experimental group and the
control group. A t-test on the subgroup Post-test TMLST Means yielded a value of
t=9.64, df=6,p<.001. This further confirms that the post-test scores of the experimental
group are significantly different than those of the control group.
53
Results of Hypothesis Testing
The results of the data analysis up to this point indicate that students in the
experimental group scored 11.42 points higher than the control group, and the difference
is statistically significant. This test calls for the rejection of the null hypothesis that
“using paintings has no effect music listening skills” and an acceptance of the research
hypothesis “Using paintings in music listening lessons will enhance musical listening
skills.”
Secondary Results
Now that we know using images of paintings in music listening instruction
enhanced music listening skills, it would be interesting to compare the effect of the two
categories of paintings, namely paintings directly related to the music or paintings
indirectly related to the music. Directly-related painting and music refer to those pieces
that, as described in chapter three, have a direct relationship and were created to
compliment or represent the music. To do this the Post-test Part B listening scores of
TMLST were first partitioned by the painting category. There were eight musical
excerpts in the TMLST, three pieces in each category and two not part of the
instrumentation. Since only the experimental group’s students viewed the images only
their scores were examined. It was found (Table 4.13) that the type of painting/music
categories had no effect on listening scores.
54
Table 4.13 Means of TMLST Post-test per Excerpt Score for Each Category
_______________________________________________
Category of Painting/Music Post-test score t-test
Directly Related 5.20 (sd=.72)
1.19(ns)
Indirectly Related 4.85 (sd=.81)
Non-instructional 4.75 (sd=.83)
________________________________________________
In addition to examining the effect of visual stimuli in listening skill, its effect on
Familiarity and on Preference was also observed. Familiarity as well as Preference was
measured for each excerpt by having the participants rate each on a scale of one to seven.
Pre-test for the control group had a mean Preference score of 3.75 (sd=1.14) and the
experimental group had a mean score of 3.69 (sd=1.06). Table 4.14 shows the means and
standard deviation for Familiarity and Preference. The mean rating for Familiarity at the
Pre-test was 1.73 (sd=1.14) for the control group and 1.75 (sd= .89) for the Experimental
Group. There was growth between the pre-test and post-test in both the control and
experimental groups in both preference and more noticeably in familiarity. For
Preference, the scores increased .51 for the Control group and .86 for the Experimental
group. Since there was no further difference found neither in the Pre-test nor the Post-
test between the Experimental and the Control Group, for Preference or Familiarity, the
increase in Pre-Post scores would be due to the effect of the instruction rather than a
difference in treatment. Statistical analysis using a paired t-test indicates that there was a
statistical significant difference between the Pre-test and Post-test scores for both the
control and experimental groups for Preference as well as Familiarity. The score for
familiarity increased more dramatically. The control group went from a mean score of
1.73 (sd= 1.02) to a mean score of 4.42 (sd=1.19) and the experimental group went from
a mean score of 1.74 (sd=.89) to a mean score of 4.35 (sd=1.29).
55
Table 4.14: Mean Rating for Preference and Familiarity
(Max 7)
Preference Pre-test Post test t-test
Control 3.80 4.31 3.25*
(sd=1.23) (sd=.92)
Experimental 3.68 4.54 3.98**
(sd=1.06) (sd=.98)
Familiarity_______________________________________
Control 1.73 4.42 14.20**
(sd=1.02) (sd=1.12)
Experimental 1.74 4.35 11.01**
(sd=.89) (sd=1.29)
_________________________________________________
*p<.01 **p<.001
The Pearson r
was used to examine the relationship between various observations.
Results are shown in Table 4.15. The correlations indicate that all Music Test Scores are
positively correlated. The TMLST had a low correlation when comparing the composite
scores of Pre-test to the Post-test for all participants (r=.199). The Pre-test/Post-test
correlation for the control group (r=.489) is much higher than that for the experimental
group (r=.091). This is evidence that the scores of control group did not change much
whereas those from the experimental group did. The correlation coefficients for the
TMLST PartA/Part B were moderate, with r=.410 for the Pre-test and r=.532 for the Post-
test. These numbers give support to TMLST as a reliable measure. Familiarity and
Preference had a positive correlation both before (r=.384) and after the instruction
(r=.622) with the correlation after the instruction being higher. There were no other
significant correlations indicating that music listening skills per se are not related to
musical preference or familiarity with the music.
56
Table 4.15 Significant Correlations
_______________________________________________
Variables Pearson r
TMLST Pre-test and Post-test .199
Control Group .489**
Experimental Group .091
Pre-test Part A and Part B .410**
Post-test Part A and Part B .532**
Familiarity and Preference
Before Instruction .384**
After Instruction .622**
Familiarity and TMLST
Pre-test all Subjects .083
Post-test all Subjects .054
Preference and TMLST
Pre-test all Subjects -.09
Post-test all Subjects .216
Familiarity and TMLST
Post-test Control Group -.07
Post-test Experimental Group .20
______________________________________________
*p<.01, **p<.001
Finally, Recall was measured as a secondary question for this study. The Recall Test had
a possible score of 32 points. The Recall test did not show any significant difference
between the control and experimental group. The control group scored a mean of 14.4
(sd=1.86) and the experimental group scored a mean of 14.6 (sd=1.66). Recall was not
affected by either instrumentation or treatment.
57
Summary
Results of Hypothesis Testing
After analysis of all the data, the null hypothesis “Using paintings in music listening
lessons has no effect on musical listening skills” is rejected. The research hypothesis
“Using paintings in music listening lessons will enhance musical listening skills” is
accepted.
The findings may be summarized as follows.
1. Instruction that involves the use of visual stimuli results in a higher post-
test score on assessments of music listening skills.
2. Familiarity of musical excerpts is increased after instruction for both
groups.
3. Preference for musical excerpts is increased after instruction for both
groups.
4. The relationship of art to music does not have to be direct to result in higher
post-test scores.
58
CHAPTER 5
DISCUSSION AND RECCOMENDATIONS
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of visual stimuli on music
listening skills in pre-service elementary teachers. The research hypothesis that “Using
paintings in music listening lessons will enhance musical listening skills” was tested and
accepted. “Visual Stimuli” refers to the presentation of arts elements in selected
paintings in the form of projected images on a screen. Subjects were pre-service
elementary general educators enrolled in a large southern university (N=93). Students
from each of four intact classes were randomly placed into either the experimental group
or the control group. The treatment consisted of six music listening lessons over a two-
week period, each group receiving the identical teaching protocol with the exception of
the use of visually projected paintings for the experimental group. Listening instruction
emphasized the identification of melodic contour, instrumentation, texture, rhythm and
expressive elements of the compositions.
The Teacher Music Listening Skills Test (TMLST) was constructed by the
investigator, piloted and administered before and after the treatment. Pre-test data for the
eight subgroups showed no difference, and the observations were consequently examined
for differences between the control group and the experimental group only. The groups
were very similar in demographics. Years of musical experience per each participant was
evenly divided between the experimental and control groups making the experimental
group and the control group as equal as possible prior to the study. Results indicate that
the group receiving visual stimuli in the form of paintings scored significantly higher on
listening skills (p<.01) than the control group which received no visual stimuli.
Part A of the TMLST pertained to basic vocabulary related to music and showed no
change between groups from pre-test to post-test, with scores of 6.02 and 6.43
respectively for the control group and 6.02 and 6.80 for the experimental group. This
would indicate that neither the treatment nor the teaching affected Part A of the TMLST.
This was as expected. Scores for Part B of the TMLST pertain to the application of
59
listening skills and they showed a significant increase for the experimental group. The
experimental group went from a mean score of 28.89 to a mean score of 40.00 which
showed a significant gain in listening skills. This would indicate that the treatment of
using visually projected images of paintings in this study was effective in teaching
listening skills. The improvement by the treatment group was measured at a t value of
10.11<p.001 suggesting a significant improvement. This data would support that the
visual treatment was successful in teaching elements such as contour, main and auxiliary
instruments, rhythm and beat structure. Preference and Familiarity were not affected by
the treatment in this study but the data does suggest that Familiarity as well as preference
was affected by the instruction in both the control and treatment groups. The control
group had a Pre-test mean score of 3.80 and the experimental group had a Pre-test mean
score of 3.68. The Post-test scores increased to 4.48 and 4.43 respectively. In the
category of Familiarity, the increase was from a mean score of 1.73 for the control and
1.74 for the experimental to 4.42 and 4.35 respectively. This is a significant increase and
suggests that the instruction and just hearing the pieces numerous times throughout the
treatment helped the students become familiar with the pieces of music regardless of the
use of visual stimuli. The gain of Preference scores increased from Pre-test to Post-test
for each group also but at a smaller margin.
Correlations were run to examine the relationship between various observations.
Correlations were run between the Pre-test and Post-test of the TMLST as well as
Familiarity and Preference and how they correlated with the TMLST. All of the
correlations moved in a positive direction, which is important because it would indicate
the success of the TMLST and the relationship of Preference and Familiarity. The
correlation between Preference and Familiarity was not terribly high, but because it is
positive it could support research done by Siebenaler (1999). He found that there was a
strong correlation between familiarity and preference in students’ learning of new songs.
The same could be suggested here as students became more familiar with pieces that data
would suggest that the preference increased as well.
Although this study was well constructed and worked well with the population it
was studying there were some threats to validity that should be addressed. During the
course of the treatment there was a severe weather event. The university and city were
60
crippled with an ice storm that closed the university for a number of days requiring
treatment days to be extended to accommodate the time lost while the university was not
holding classes. This had the potential of adversely affecting the study by delaying the
time between presented excerpts and Post-testing, extending the time between the excerpt
presentation and the post test beyond what the original study had called for. There was
also some mortality during this study. Although the total sample for the study was
sufficient to show significant results that can be inferred to a larger population of pre-
service educators, a total of nine subjects were removed from the study for various
reasons mentioned previously in this text. In addition, there was some concern that
subjects in the control group would interact with those subjects in the experimental
group. Students in each group interacted with each other before and after each treatment
as they arrived and left from class. The researcher did not instruct the students that they
were not to discuss what was happening in each class session for fear that it would
heighten the subjects’ awareness that the treatments were different. When a student
asked if all students were hearing the same music or seeing the same paintings, they were
told that everyone in their group saw and heard the same thing. To reduce experimenter
effects, scripts were followed implicitly as mentioned earlier in research. Although none
of these threats to validity was overt enough to alter the results it is important to consider
what can be improved for similar future studies.
The current study attempted to address the need and importance for an aesthetic
education through listening. Broudy (1958) stated that the ability to detect aesthetic form
(the arrangement of elements that attracts, holds and directs the interests of the listener) is
at the heart of music education. This still holds true today and this study was designed to
direct the listener to identify elements that make up the aesthetic form by using, in the
case of the experimental group, a multi-sensory approach. Listeners were directed to use
visual images in the form of paintings. The results here are in agreement that students
were more successful identifying some of the elements needed to detect aesthetic form
after a treatment with a multi-sensory approach. Berleant (1991) argued for a
participatory aesthetic, emphasizing engagement and contemplation with participation.
The treatment in this study encouraged active engagement. Anecdotal reports were made
informally and showed evidence that students interacted with the music and art as well as
61
continued use of art and music after the end of the treatment implying that students were
actively engaged in listening. The Getty Foundation and the National Endowment for the
Arts stand by the position that arts education should include aesthetics as well as
performance and production. Developing successful listening lessons such as the ones
found in this study exposes students to the aesthetic element and would support the
position held by these two organizations. Students were given an opportunity to listen to
and study musical excerpts creating the opportunity to develop an aesthetic view of the
piece.
The findings in this study support the argument that people learn in a variety of
ways. Research by Park (2000) suggests that when teaching secondary education
students, multi-sensory instructional materials should be presented. The data of this
study would suggest that students, given multi-sensory instructional materials, are more
successful than those students who received instructional material aurally only. Students
given multi-sensory instruction using paintings scored higher on the TMLST than those
students who did not receive lessons in a multi-sensory way. This study also reinforces
work done by Kalyuga, Chandler and Sweller (2000) suggesting that the amount of
information being processed using both auditory and visual channels is significantly
larger than the amount of information processed using only a single channel. Previous
research has also suggested the importance of using an integrated approach to teaching by
engaging more than one of the senses. This was reinforced by this study with the
statistically significant scores by those subjects that were presented musical information
as well as visual stimuli utilizing the senses of both sight and hearing.
This study addressed the specific learning needs of the population participating in
the study. Students at the secondary and adult level have specific needs. The treatment
followed the need to be well managed with regard to time, relevant to the students in a
practical manner, intensive in material presentation and short in nature. It can be inferred
by the positive results of this study that the subjects responded well to the model
presented for teaching adults. Students were receptive to the material presented and as
shown by the data in the Pre-test and Post-test, the scores reflect the success of the model.
The subjects also responded as expected with regard to what they listened for. Studies
conducted by Madsen and Geringer (1990) suggested that non-musicians listen for
62
specific things such as instrumentation, timbre and rhythm. The current study supports
the research by Madsen and Geringer. Subjects did indeed listen for instrumentation,
timbre and rhythm accurately. Recall was also measured as secondary data collection.
The data did not show any difference between the control and experimental groups
suggesting that possibly the treatment or teaching did not affect recall of musical features
or names associated with this piece. The testing instrument for Recall here was designed
to observe the students’ ability to recall composer, composition and painting by using
open-ended questions in order to avoid giving cues. The responses suggest that such
open-ended questions failed to trigger the retrieval of the target information.
The research utilized the Gestalt Theory and more specifically the Gestalt Law of
Similarity as the basis. Gestalt theory is the “organization of sensory information into
meaningful whole based on experience” (Swanick, 1988). This study presented sensory
information in the form of visual and aural sensory stimuli via paintings and music.
Students were then guided to create a meaningful whole in the context of using listening
lessons and in the case of the experimental group with the addition of projected images of
paintings. Increased Post-test scores provided the clue that students were probably
successful in creating a meaningful whole. After the completion of the treatment,
students in both the control and experimental groups were able to answer questions
regarding an excerpt of music better than they could prior to the study, thus suggesting
that the musical excerpts had become meaningful for them. The degree of success is
reflected in the amount of improvement by each group indicating that the subjects in the
experimental group did much better. The indication is that using visual stimuli with aural
stimuli aided in making the musical excerpts more meaningful. The use of the Gestalt
Law of Similarity alone was to narrow the scope of the research to a manageable size
given time and class restraints. The Gestalt Law of Similarity was used successfully for
the selection of art and music pieces with elements that exemplify possible groupings
with similar attributes. The findings here suggest that the different manner of similarity
is not crucial. Paintings and music with a direct relationship or having a strong similarity
in characteristics scored the same as those art and music examples that had an indirect
relationship. This seems to imply that specific associations are not as important as
63
providing opportunities for increased sensory organization in proactive music listening
skills.
A secondary outcome of this study was made possible by the observations
collected in a journal kept by the researcher. During the course of the two weeks of
treatment the researcher kept a journal of qualitative notes for both the control and
treatment group. The journal was used to document things including how often students
offered their own insights or comments about the music and or the art work, how often
students asked questions regarding the art, music or the composer, and how interested the
subjects were in the music and in the case of the experimental group, the art work. After
examining the anecdotal information found in the journal it was discovered that the
subgroups participating in the experimental treatment were more vocal during class,
asked more questions during the presentation and used the correct vocabulary more often
when describing each of the elements when answering questions in class than the control
group. Students in the experimental group also showed a great deal of interest in the
paintings as a stand-alone item, asking questions about the artist, medium and time
period. Although this was not observed in the control group, it is important to note the
level of interest in the paintings the students in the experimental group exhibited. Lastly,
several students in the experimental group expressed motivation to use listening in their
future classrooms as an integrated activity. Several students in the experimental group
sought out information to integrate art and music for various projects in other
methodology classes outside of music.
There were also several observations of the study participants after the
conclusion of the two weeks of treatment. Students in the elementary music methods
class are required to complete and teach a listening lesson similar to the ones presented
during the treatment by the end of the semester. Students this year, regardless of
placement in the control or treatment groups, had better lesson presentations and a more
effective delivery than what has been observed in the same class in years passed.
Regardless of being assigned to control or experimental groups, students used correct
music vocabulary to describe the listening correctly more often and more accurately in
their own listening presentations. The listening lessons that students presented this year
were also very detailed. Students addressed what to listen for within the lesson and
64
students utilized multiple learning modalities to teach selected piece of music. Because
there is no hard data to support these observations, this data cannot be included in the
scope of this study, but I feel that it is important to mention the effect the study had on
other aspects of what the students were doing during over the course of the semester.
Recommendations for Further Research
There is a great deal still to be examined in the field of using art to teach aspects
of music. Art and music, although often associated, are rarely studied together or used as
a tool to teach skills in the other. Opportunities for further research include running this
same study using other Gestalt Laws to relate the art work and the music. This study
only considers the Gestalt Law of Similarity but there is a significant amount of data to
be collected using Proximity, Common Fate or Good Continuation to relate the art and
music to each other. Data from similar studies using proximity, common fate or good
continuation could add strength to the argument that the Gestalt Theory is effective in
relationship to music.
It would also be beneficial to examine these same effects on different learning
populations including school-aged children at different developmental levels, musicians
and the elderly. Using different populations with the same independent and dependent
variable could ultimately strengthen the argument for using multi-sensory modalities to
teach music, regardless of population or musical training. The measurement of recall did
not yield any useful information in this particular study, but the measurement of recall
using visual stimuli should be studied, especially within the parameters of recall in music.
The independent and dependent variable could also be manipulated to include other
music skills besides listening, and other forms of art work besides paintings. The results
of this study suggests that the art work presented does not have to directly relate to the
music to be effective; the same may also prove to be true with multiple mediums of art.
Finally this study could also be reversed and music could be used to help teach aspects of
an art lesson.
65
Implications for Educational Practice
As mentioned previously, overall students in both groups seemed to be much
more comfortable with the material that they were teaching during their assigned
listening lessons after the completion of the treatment than in previous years, and
research such as Kvet and Watkins (1993) showed that pre-service teachers who are
comfortable with material are more likely to use it in their future classroom than those
that are not comfortable with new concepts or curricula. This would suggest that the pre-
service teachers in this study might be more apt to integrate listening into their general
curricula in future classroom situations. This would support the importance of this study
with regard to teaching pre-service teachers.
The findings of this study confirmed previous research findings that the visual
stimuli or paintings do not have to have a strong similarity or direct relationship with the
music presented to enhance listening. The success of both the directly-related paintings
and the indirectly-related paintings would suggest that using images can be effective and
should be included in music lessons in future classrooms. Regardless of a direct or
indirect relationship with art and music, one of the concerns here apart form the aesthetic
value, was to ascertain that the art works used should be accessible to educators. The
pool of 15 paintings that the researcher began with was picked for accessibility, aesthetic
reaction and by educational content decided by the researcher. These paintings were then
ranked by professional artists and an examination of each painting’s history was
conducted. Paintings ranged from strongly related to not at all; from those paintings, six
high scoring paintings were used. The research shows that the treatment was successful
regardless of the ranking of the paintings with regard to similarity and relationship. The
researcher for this study did not have a great deal of training in art when she picked the
art work, which would suggest it is not crucial that there be a deep understanding of art to
accomplish this or similar studies. The key is to research its origin and obtain
information for each of the art pieces, and examine its relationship to the music that it
may accompany. With the readily available resources on the internet, art work is
accessible for any teacher, without the need for slides or access to an art museum. Basic
art knowledge can be gained from a variety of sources including art specialists, books and
66
websites and information on the origin and history of a painting is as easily accessible
from the same sources.
In conclusion, the present research confirms that using images of paintings can
enhance music listening skills in adults and should be included in teacher preparation
programs. Such effect on children needs to be researched in the near future. This study
is only near the beginning of what can be researched in the field of Gestalt and music and
using art in teaching music skills. It has reinforced both the relationship between Gestalt
and music and that of art and music listening. This study would serve as a catalyst in
developing more research that will strengthen the relationship between art and music in
teaching music skills. There is a great deal of research still to be done in both Gestalt and
music, and art and music.
67
APPENDIX A TMLST
PLEASE ANSWER ALL QUESTIONS. PLEASE GUESS IF YOU ARE NOT SURE
Part A- Matching
A-I Expressive Elements
Match the expressive element with its symbol. Circle the correct letter name on the
answer sheet.
1. Accent a. e.
2. Slur
3. Staccato b. f.
4. Decrescendo
5. Fermata c. g.
6. Repeat sign
d. h.
A-II Form
Match the Musical Form with its pattern. Circle the correct letter name on the answer
sheet. ( Questions 7-10)
7. Binary a. ABA
8. Rondo b. BBAB
9. Theme and Variation c. ABAB
10. Ternary d. ABACADA
e. A1A2A3A4
Part B- Listening
For each musical excerpt you are to listen to, study the answer sheet and answer the question
during the playing of the excerpt. Place all responses on the separate Answer Sheet.
a. Name the instrument or instruments playing the main melody.
b. Circle all other instrument or instruments you hear.
c. Identify the texture. Circle the appropriate type.
d. Choose an answer that best represents the piece.
i) What can you say about the rhythmic motive?
ii) What is the predominant group of beats?
iii) What is the predominant tempo?
iv) What is the overall articulation?
e. Study the following patterns and identify the contour that best represents the
main melody as part of the excerpt is played again
.
Circle the appropriate number on the answer sheet
f. On a scale of 1-7, how familiar
is the piece of music to you?
(1 being not at all, 7 being very familiar)
g. On a scale of 1-7 how well do you like the piece of music?
(1 being not at all, 7 being very much)
68
TMLST
Answer Sheet
Part A- Matching
A-I Expressive Elements
Match the expressive element with its symbol. Circle the correct letter name.
1. Accent a b c d e f g h
2. Slur a b c d e f g h
3. Staccato a b c d e f g h
4. Decrescendo a b c d e f g h
5. Fermata a b c d e f g h
6. Repeat Sign a b c d e f g h
A-II Form
Match the Musical Form with its pattern. Circle the correct letter name.
7. Binary a b c d e
8. Rondo a b c d e
9. Theme and
Variation a b c d e
10. Ternary a b c d e
69
Part B- Listening
Excerpt # 1
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Full Orchestra Strings Woodwinds Percussion
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i..) ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
70
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #2
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Timpani Bassoon Piccolo Violin
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
71
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #3
a. Main Instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Woodwinds Full Orchestra Tambourine Clarinet
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
72
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #4
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Rhythm Instruments Piano Low brass Clarinet
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
73
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #5
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Flute Trumpet Strings Bass drum
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
74
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #6
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Percussion Horn Oboe Brass
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
75
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #7
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Full Orchestra Rhythm Instruments Bassoon Horn
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
76
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #8
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Woodwinds Percussion Full Orchestra Brass
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
Rhythm: i) even uneven syncopated Repetition of
Rhythmic motive
Grouping
Of beats: ii) Duple Triple Compound Irregular
Tempo: iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
Articulation iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
77
APPENDIX B- Recall Test
Name _________________________
Section________________________
This exercise is to assess your ability to identify musical elements from listening to music.
Your careful responses will provide me with the feedback about the unit on listening.
Instruction: You will hear several excerpts of musical composition. After listening to each musical
excerpt, please answer the questions to the best of your ability. You will be given time after each
excerpt to write your answers. The excerpts will be played only once.
In your opinion, how do you feel about the listening unit? (choose one response and circle)
I would like more lessons in music listening
I think I learned enough for now
I think the listening unit is too long
Excerpt #1
1. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
2. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
3. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
78
Excerpt #2
4. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
5. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
6. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
Excerpt #3
7. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
8. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
79
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
9. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
Excerpt #4
10. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
11. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
12. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
80
Excerpt #5
13. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
14. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
15. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
Excerpt #6
16. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
17. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
81
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
18. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
Excerpt #7
19. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
20. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
21. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
82
Excerpt #8
22. Have you ever heard the piece before? ( circle yes or no below)
YES NO
23. Describe this piece as best as you can. Use what you have learned during the
listening lessons; include the usage of as many musical elements as possible.
o Title (if known)____________________________________________
o Composer (if known)________________________________________
o Musical elements _______________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o _________________________________________________________
o ________________________________________________________
24. What Visual image comes to mind as you listen to this piece?
____________________________________________________________
_____________________________________________________________
83
APPENDIX C - Permission
I understand that in taking this MUS 261 course, some of the regular work and
assignments will be used as data for a research study related to a dissertation on effective
teaching techniques. My grades will not be affected in any way and my name will not be
mentioned anywhere. If I have any concerns or reservations I will contact my instructor
as soon as possible.
Signed__________________________________________
Date____________________________________________
84
APPENDIX D
85
APPENDIX E-Sample Listening Lesson Scripts
Goal: To enhance listening by using musical elements and in the case of the experimental
group, to enforce listening.
Objective: Students will be able to identify and recognize salient musical elements that
will assist in the identification and categorization of musical excerpts.
Lesson will run 25 minutes:
Attendance and class announcement are to be made at the beginning of each treatment
and are not to exceed 3 minutes.
The Musical excerpt is introduced by writing the title and composer’s name on
the dry wipe board.
Title of composition is read out loud and the composers name is read out loud.
Birth and Death dates are given for the composer to give context to a time frame
for the composition.
Events happening at the same time in history are pointed out, Beethoven, and the
French revolution, American Revolution etc.
Biographical information about the composer is presented verbally, with no
visual cues or prompts. Ex: Beethoven became deaf and was not a product of a
patronage system.
The Composition is introduced in context to the composer’s life, when it was
written, why, and how long it took.
Excerpt of the composition is played in it’s entirety without further discussion.
Students are not asked to listen to any salient musical elements in particular.
After the piece is listened to once, a list of seven musical elements is written on
the board below the Composer and Composition title. The seven elements are:
1. Main Instrument or instruments
2. Auxiliary instruments
3. Contour
4. Rhythm
5. Beat Structure
6. Texture
7. Articulation
These elements are written in this order on the board. Students are invited to
comment on each element offering their opinion and insight as to what they
heard on the first time they have heard the piece.
After students have been asked to fill in the blanks without prompting, the
instructor asks pointed questions to guide the listeners to what they heard.
The piece is played a second time, with interjections where necessary to point
out each element listed on the board as it becomes clear.
86
After the second time the piece is heard students are asked to contribute to the
list in any further way they would like to.
After listening to the piece a second time, in the case of the experimental
group a painting assigned to the excerpt was projected in front of the students
and where applicable portions of the painting was used to reinforce salient
musical elements. For example, in Gottschalk and Suerat, the style of
painting or pointillism reinforces the articulation of the piece.
Students were invited to ask any questions about the excerpt, and in the case
of the experimental group, the paintings.
The piece of music was played a third and final time without discussion or
interruption for both groups.
Class was dismissed.
87
APPENDIX F- Concepts Taught During Listening Lessons.
Lesson One-
A Night in the Tropics by L. Gottschalk. The painting used is Sunday on the Island of La
Grande Jatte by G. Suerat.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. Articulation- associated with the style of painting called pointillism.
2. Contour- associated with the form and how the eye travels from one element
to the next returning to the beginning. The contour is repetitive and returns to
the beginning of the excerpt.
3. Texture- associated with the alternating use of light and dark areas of the
painting with the thick and thinly orchestrated sections of the musical excerpt.
Lesson Two-
Soldier’s Tale by Stravinsky. The painting to be used is Fugue by W. Kandinsky.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. Instrumentation, Main and Auxiliary- the association of specific instruments
both carrying the main melody and in auxiliary roles and the use of color for
the representation of each instrument timbre.
2. Texture- The association of the placement of color upon color in association
with the blending of timbres.
88
Lesson Three-
Contrupunctus #2 by J.S. Bach. The painting used was Fugue by Ciurlionis.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. Contour- The shapes of the pine trees represent the shape of this melody.
2. Texture- The painting is a direct representation of the texture of a fugue
3. Instruments, Main and Auxiliary. - The painting represents the entrances of
each of the instruments in the fugue and the shape of the fugue with its use of
altered subjects.
Lesson Four-
Symphony #9 the fifth movement by L. von Beethoven. The painting used was Beethoven
Frieze by Klimt.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. The overall painting was Klimt interpretation of this particular musical
excerpt.
2. Texture- Represented in the color and spacing used in the painting
3. Contour- The shapes used in the painting represents the contour of the main
melody.
Lesson Five-
Symphony #94 Movement 2 by Haydn. The painting used was La Dance by Henri
Matisse.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
89
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. Contour- The shapes of Matisse are simple and can be associated with the
simple lines of the Matisse images of dancing individuals.
2. Articulation- The articulation of the piece of music is clean with no
ambiguities; the images in the paintings along with the color convey the same
idea.
3. Beat Structure- The beat structure of the music is straight forward with no
unusual differences and can be represented by the construction of the images
in the painting.
Lesson Six-
Valse Triste by Sibelius. The painting used is Untitled by Paul Klee.
Elements to be discussed:
1. Main Instrument or Instruments
2. Auxiliary Instruments
3. Rhythm
4. Beat Structure
5. Texture
6. Articulation
7. Contour
Dominant art elements present in the painting and associated with musical elements.
1. Texture- The excerpt has a very dense texture with multiple timbres being
sounded at the same time. The painting can be associated with the dense
musical texture by the very dense imagery and use of colors in the painting.
2. Beat Structure- The excerpt has an indeterminate beat structure and the
painting conveys an indeterminate imagery.
90
APPENDIX G- Art Survey
Dear Professor/Artist.
I, Jennifer Shank, appreciate your willingness to help as an expert judge regarding some
art works. I am working on my dissertation that researches the use of painting as a
catalyst to learn about music composition. Specifically, I hope that pointing out
similarities in painting might stimulate listeners to listen for similarities in music. Your
expertise will enable me to select art works that are good examples of containing
“similarity” within the artwork itself.
According to the Gestalt principles, the definition of Similarity is that “Items similar in
some respect tend to be grouped together”. In the case of painting, I’d like to have your
opinion about the extent of similarity for each of the art elements or principles in a
specific painting.
Enclosed you will find a CD rom with slides of 11 art works. Please use the enclosed
rating sheets to rate each of the art works presented. When you have finished please
return the packet to either Dr. Cecilia Wang’s mailbox or mine in room 105 of the fine
arts building. Thank you in advance for your time and effort.
Sincerely,
Jennifer Shank
91
Please rate each of the following paintings on similarity for each of the following artistic
elements and principles in the scale of 1-10.
Please feel free to comment or explain your rating if you wish.
1 denotes No Similarity
10 denotes Very Strong Evidence of Similarity
Painting # Slide # Comments
Color
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Form
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Shape
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Texture
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Balance
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Pattern
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Rhythm
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Thank you very much!!
92
APPENDIX H- Pilot Study
Pilot Study
Prior to the beginning of this study, a pilot study was run to test the feasibility of
the research and the researcher-created pretest and posttests. The pilot was run with
music education majors and non-musicians from a nearby university, and non music
majors in a music appreciation class at this university. The pilot study was run over a
course of 5 days with one day for the pretest and the first day of treatment, a second day
of treatment, and the music knowledge portion of the posttest on the third meeting. The
results, although not significant, did suggest that the use of visual stimuli in the form of
paintings could enhance musical knowledge in the areas of texture and instrumentation,
melodic contour and form.
Subjects
The subjects for the pilot study were students enrolled in music appreciation
classes and elementary and middle school education methods classes at Campbellsville
University, as well as students enrolled in a music appreciation class at the University of
Kentucky. Both music appreciation classes and elementary education methods classes
were used due to class size and class availability. Students in all of the classes used were
untrained, or non-musicians. The total population for the study was n=86. Classes were
randomly assigned to the control or treatment groups.
93
Procedure
The pretest was administered prior to the treatment session during the first
meeting. The pretest took approximately 15 minutes to administer. After the pretest, the
treatment was administered. For this study, the treatment was 25 minutes long and one
excerpt was presented each treatment. each session. Excerpts were first presented by
introducing the piece with pertinent composer information and the title. The excerpt was
then played once for the subjects without interruption and without instructions to listen
for specific elements. Students then received approximately 10 minutes of lecture with
the use of call charts and, in the case of the experimental group, paintings to reinforce the
musical concepts being covered. During the lecture portion of the treatment, portions of
the excerpt were played to reinforce specific points. Students in both the control and
experimental groups were given identical information with the exception of the paintings
for the experimental group. The students then heard the excerpt in its entirety two more
times in succession. The first time the researcher pointed out features as the music was
played. The second time, the music was played without interruption or discussion. For
each excerpt, instrumentation was discussed. Depending on the strength of melodic
contour texture, or form within the excerpt, those concepts were featured as well. For
excerpt one and melodic contour and texture was stressed as well as instrumentation. For
excerpt three and four, form was stressed along with instrumentation. Excerpts one and
two were presented on treatment day one and excerpts three and four were presented
during treatment day two.
94
The posttest was administered on a subsequent class meeting after all four
excerpts were presented. Students were aware there would be a posttest but were not
asked to prepare for it in any particular way.
Music and Artwork Used in Pilot Study
The music and artwork chosen for the pilot study were from selections being
considered for the research study. All of the music was instrumental in nature, with
similar instrumentation. Excerpts were purposely chosen to not be immediately
recognizable and each excerpt was no longer than three minutes long. The following
excerpts were used: 1. Symphony No.4 in A Major, Opus 90 “Italian” Movement 2
Andante Con moto by Felix Mendelssohn. 2. Symphony, “A Night in the Tropics”
Movement One, Andante by Louise Moreau Gottschalk. 3. “The Soldier’s Tale” The
Soldiers March by Igor Stravinsky. 4. Symphony No.2, Opus 43 in D Major, Movement
One, Allegretto by Jean Sibelius. Music was chosen based on instrumentation and the
music’s association with visual art or stimuli. The artwork chosen for the experimental
group was chosen after consulting art faculty and measuring the element of similarity
apparent in each work. Paintings used included works by George Suerat, Mendelssohn,
Kandinsky, and Goya.
Pretest and Posttest
The pretest and posttest were researcher-created and designed to measure musical
knowledge and preference. It is important to note that the posttest was revised several
times and administered a second time after flaws were found in the first test design. The
95
post test was given with familiar excerpts and unfamiliar excerpts with a total of five
excerpts in all. Both the pretest and posttest had a possible score of 25 points.
96
PILOT PRETEST AND POSTTEST
Part A- Matching
A-I Expressive Elements
Match the expressive element with its symbol. Circle the correct letter name on the
answer sheet.
10. Accent a.
11. Slur b.
12. staccato c.
13. decrescendo d.
14. fermata e.
15. repeat sign f
g
h.
A-II Form
Match the Musical Form with its pattern. Circle the correct letter name on the answer
sheet. ( Questions 7-10)
16. Binary a. ABA
17. Rondo b. BBAB
18. Theme and Variation c. ABAB
10. Ternary d. ABACADA
e. A1A2A3A4
Part B- Listening
For each musical excerpt you are to listen, place all responses on the separate Answer Sheet.
h. Name the instrument or instruments playing the main melody.
i. Name another instrument or instruments you hear.
j. Identify the texture. Circle the appropriate type.
k. Choose an answer that best represents the piece.
i) What can you say about the rhythm?
ii) What is the predominant meter?
iii) What is the predominant tempo?
iv) What is the predominant articulation?
v) What is the overall dynamic level?
l. Study the following patterns and identify the contour that best represents the
main melody as part of the excerpt is played again.
Circle the appropriate number on the answer sheet
m. On a scale of 1-7, how familiar is the piece of music to you?
(1 being not at all, 7 being very familiar)
n. On a scale of 1-7 how well do you like the piece of music?
(1 being not at all, 7 being very much)
97
Name_________________________
Mus 100, Section_________
Date__________________________
Answer Sheet
Part A- Matching
A-I Expressive Elements
Match the expressive element with its symbol. Circle the correct letter name.
Accent 1. a b c d e f g h
Slur 2. a b c d e f g h
Staccato 3. a b c d e f g h
Decrescendo 4. a b c d e f g h
Fermata 5. a b c d e f g h
Repeat Sign 6. a b c d e f g h
A-II Form
Match the Musical Form with its pattern. Circle the correct letter name.
Binary 7. a b c d e
Rondo 8. a b c d e
Theme and
Variation 9. a b c d e
Ternary 10. a b c d e
98
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #1
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)______________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i..) ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
99
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #2
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
100
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #3
a. Main Instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
101
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #4
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instruments(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
102
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #5
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
103
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #6
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
104
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #7
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
105
Part B- Listening
Excerpt #8
a. Main instrument (s)______________________________________________
b. Other instrument(s)________________________________________________
c. Circle only one: Homophonic Polyphonic Heterophonic Monophonic
d. Circle only one choice for each item:
i) even uneven syncopated lots of repetition
ii) 2/4 3/4 4/4 6/8
iii) Allegro Moderato Presto Largo
iv) Staccato Legato Pizzicato Marcato
v) pp f mp ff
e. Choose one contour:
i.). ii.)
f. Not Familiar at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very Familiar
g. Do Not Like at All 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Like Very Much
106
REFERENCES
Anglin, G.J. (1986). Cognitive psychology and its implications 3
rd
Ed. New York,
NY: Freeman.
Arnheim, R. (1954,1974). Art and visual perception: a psychology of the creative
eye. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Beethoven, J., Bohn, D., Hiley, M. et.al. (1995). The music connection.
Parsippany, NJ: Silver Burdett.
Beethoven, J., Brumfield, S., Campbell, P.S et al. (2002). Making music.
Parsippany, NJ: Scott Foresman.
Berleant, A. (1991). Art and engagement. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
Berlin, S. R. ( 1976). Verbal and musical facilitation of vividness of mental
imagery, A comprehensive study. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, California School
of Professional Psychology: Fresno.
Bharucha, J. J. (1994). Tonality and expectation. In R. Aiello & J. Sloboda (Eds.),
Musical Perceptions (pp. 215-239). New York: Oxford University Press.
Blatnik, F. (1988). Study confirms teaching with videodisc beats textbook
methods. T.H.E. Journal, 16, 58-60.
Blocher, L.R. (1990). An analysis of college band instrumentalists’ detection of
common performance errors. Paper presented at the Southern Division CBDNA/NBA
Conference, Tallahassee, FL.
Boardman, E. (2001). Generating a theory of music instruction. Music Educators
Journal. 88, 2, 45-53.
Boardman, E. & Andress, B. (1981). The Music Book. New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart, and Winston.
Bowman, W. (1969). Philosophy, criticism and music education: some tentative
steps down a less traveled road. Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education,
114, 1-19.
Boyer-White, R., Campbelle-Dugard, M., de Frece, R., et.al.(1995). Share the
music. New York: McGraw Hill.
107
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: the perceptual organization of
sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bresler, L. (1993). Music in a double bind: Instruction by non-specialists in
elementary schools. Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education, 115, 1-13.
Briggs, K. C. & Myers, I. B. (1977). Myers-Briggs type indicator. Palo Alto,
California: Consulting Psychologist Press.
Broudy, H. S. (1958). A realistic philosophy of music education. In N.B. Henry
(Ed.), Basic Concepts in Music Education.(146-181). Chicago: National Society for the
Study of Education.
Burgess, N. & Hitch, G.J. (1996). A connectionist model of STM for serial order.
Psychological Review, 106, 551-581.
Campbell, M.R. (1991). Musical learning and the development of psychological
processes in perception and cognition. Bulletin for the Council on Research in Music
Education, 107, 35-42.
Clarke, E.R. (1989). Mind the gap: formal structures and psychological processes
in music. Contemporary Music Review, 3, 1-13.
Consortium of National Arts Education Association. (1994). National standards
for arts education: what every young American should know and be able to do in the arts.
Reston, VA: Music Educators National Conference.
Craik, F.M. & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and retention of works in
episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 104, 268-293.
Cutietta, R.A. (1993). The musical elements, Who said they’re right? Music
Educators Journal, 79, 48-53.
Darrow, A.A. (1990). The role of hearing in understanding music. Music
Educators Journal. 77, 4, 24-28.
Deliege, I. (1987). Grouping conditions in listening to music: an approach to
Lerdahl and Jackendoff’s grouping preferences rules. Music Perception, 4, 325-360.
Deliege, I. (1989). A perceptual approach to contemporary musical forms, in S.
McAdams & I. Deliege (Eds.) Music and Cognitive Science (Special Issue.)
Contemporary Music Review, 4, 213-230.
108
Deliege, I. & El Ahmadi, A. (1990). Mechanisms of cue abstraction in musical
groupings: A study of perception of Sequenza IV for viola solo by Luciano Berio.
Psychology of Music, 18, 18-44.
Deutsch, D. (1982, 1999). Grouping mechanisms in music. In D. Deutsch (Ed.)
The psychology of music. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dewey, J. (1958). Art as experience. New York: G.P Putnam’s Sons.
Dewitt, L.A. & Crowder, R.G. (1986). Recognition of novel melodies after brief
delays. Music Perception 3 p. 259-274.
Dickey, J.P. & Hendricks, R.C. (1991). Visual perception qualities of
instructional materials. The Clearing House, 64, 168-170.
Dowling, W.J. & Bartlett, J. C. ( 1981). The importance of interval information
in long term memory. In S.W. Ye (Ed.) Music Mind and Science. (pp. 166-188). Seoul:
Seoul National University Press.
Dowling, W.J. (1994). Melodic contour in hearing and remembering melodies. In
R. Aiello & J. A. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical Perceptions (pp. 173-190). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Dowling, W.J. & Harwood, D.L. (1986). Music cognition. Orlando, FL:
Academic Press.
Dowling, W.J, Tillmann, B. & D. Ayers. (2001). Memory and the experience of
hearing music. Music Perception 19, 2, 249-276.
Drago, M. (1993). College musicianship: does it work? Music Educators Journal,
79, 40-43.
Dubos, A. (1715, 1748). Critical reflections on poetry and painting. Reprint from
the fifth edition. New York: AMS Press.
Dunn, R. & Dunn, K. (1978). Teaching students through their individual learning
styles. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Dunn. R & Dunn, K. (1993). Teaching secondary students through their
individual learning styles: practical approaches for grades 7-12. Boston: Allyn and
Bacon.
Eisner, E. W. (1998). The kind of schools we need: personal essays. Portsmouth,
NJ: Heinemann Press.
109
Elliott, D. J. (1995). Music matters: a new philosophy of music education. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Elliott, D.J. (1990). Music as culture: toward a multi-cultural concept of art
education. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 24 (1), 147-166.
Erber, N.P. & Hirsh, I. ( 1978). Auditory training. In H. Davis & S.R. Silverman.
(Eds.) Hearing and Deafness. (pp.358-361). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Fischer, B. & Fischer, L. (1979). Styles in teaching and learning. Educational
Leadership, 36, 245-254.
Forsythe, J.L. & Kelly, M.M. (1989). Effects of visual-spatial added cues on
fourth-graders’ melodic discrimination. Journal of Research in Music Education. 37, 4,
272-277.
Fry, R. (1920). Vision and design. Cleveland: World (1956 Printing).
Gardner, H. (1990). Art education and human development. Los Angeles: Getty
Center for Education in the Arts.
Geringer, J.M. & Madsen, C. F. (1995). Focus of attention to elements and
listening patterns of musicians and non-musicians. Bulletin for the Council of Research
in Music Education, 127, 80-87.
Goehr, A. (1990). Music as communication. In D.H. Mellor (Ed.) Ways of
Communicating. (pp.125-142). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Goldberg, T. & Schrack, G. (1986). Computer-aided correlation of musical and
visual structures. Leonardo, 19 (1),11-17.
Goodman, N. ( 1968). Languages of Art. Indianapolis: Bobbs Merrill.
Gordon, I.E. (1989). Theories of visual perception. New York: John Wiley and
Sons.
Grasha, A.F. & Reichmann, S.W. (1975). Student learning styles questionnaire.
Cincinnati, OH: University of Cincinnati Faculty Resource Center.
Green, L. (1988). Music on deaf ears. Manchester, U.K.: Manchester University
Press.
Greene, M. (1986). The spaces of aesthetic education. Journal of Aesthetic
Education, 20 (4), 56-62.
Gregorc, A.F. (1982). Gregorc style delineator. Maynard, MA: Gabriel Systems.
110
Grey, T. (1997). Tableaux vivants: landscape, history, painting and the visual
imagination in Mendelssohn’s orchestral music. Nineteenth Century Music, 21, 38-76.
Haack, P. (1970). A study involving the visual arts in the development of musical
concepts. Journal of Research in Music Education, 18, 392-398.
Haack, P. (1992). The acquisition of music listening skills in R. Colwell (Ed.)
Handbook of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 451-465). New York:
Schirmer Books.
Hair, H. I. ( 1993). Descriptive vocabulary and visual choices: Children’s
responses to conceptual changes in music. Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music
Education, 91,59- 69.
Hair, H.I. (1995). Children’s descriptions and representations of music. Bulletin
for the Council of Research in Music Education, 119, 41-48.
Halpern, A. R, Bartlett, J.C. & W. J. Dowling ( 1998). The perception of mode
rhythm and contour in unfamiliar melodies: Effects of age and experience. Music
Perception, 15 (4), 335-355.
Hill, J.E. (1976) The educational sciences. Bloomfield Hills, MI: Oakland
Community College Press.
Hitch, G.J., Burgess, N., Towse, J.N. & Culpin, V. (1996). Temporal grouping
effects in immediate recall: A working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Section A- Human Experimental Psychology, 49 (1), 116-139.
Hoffer, C.R. (1979). A concise introduction to music listening, 2
nd
ed. Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth Publishing Co.
Issing, L.J., Hanneman, J. & Haack, J. (1989). Visualization by pictorial
analogies in understanding expository text. In H. Mandl and J. R. Levin, (Eds.).
Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures. Amsterdam: North- Holland.
Jung, C. (1927). The theory of psychological type. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Kagan, A. (1986). Ut pictura musica, 1to 1860. Arts Magazine, 60 (9), 86-91.
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (2000). Incorporating learner experience
into the design of multimedia instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 (1), 126-
136.
111
Keefe, J.W.(1979). Learning style overview student learning styles. Reston, VA:
National Association of Secondary School Principals.
Kepes, G. (Ed.) (1965). Education and vision: vision and value series vol 1.
Taiwan: Central Book.
Klein, J. T. (1990). Interdisciplinary history, theory, and practice. Detroit, MI,
Wayne State University Press.
Kolb, D. A. (1981). Learning styles and disciplinary differences. In A.W.
Chickering (Ed.), The modern college (pp. 231-255). San Francisco: Jossey- Bass.
Koniari, D., Predazzer, S. & Melen, M. ( 2001). Categorization and
schematization process used in music perception by 10-11 year old children. Music
Perception, 18 (3), 297-324.
Krumhansl, C.L. (1996). A perceptual analysis of Mozart’s Piano Sonata:
segmentation, tension, and musical ideas. Music Perception, 13, 401-432.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990) Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Kvet, E.J. & Watkins, R.C. ( 1993). Success attributes in teaching music as
perceived by elementary-education majors. Journal of Research in Music Education. 41,
1, 70-80.
Langer, S.K. (1957). Philosophy in a new key, 3
rd
Ed. Cambridge, Massachusetts
and London, England: Harvard University Press.
Leonhard, C. (1991). Status of arts education in American public schools: report
on a survey conducted by the national arts education research center at the University of
Illinois. Evanston, IL: University of Illinois.
Leonhard, C. & House, R.W. (1972). Foundations and principles of music
education, 2
nd
ed. New York: McGraw Hill, Inc.
Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983, 1996). A generative theory of tonal music.
Cambridge: MIT Press.
Levie, W. H. and Lentz, R. (1982). The Effect of text illustrations: a review of
research. Educational Communication and Technology Journal, 30,195-232.
112
Levie, W.H. (1987). Research in pictures: A guide to the literature. In D.M.
Willows & H. A. Haughton, (Eds.), The psychology of illustration. Volume 1: Basic
Research. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Levinson, J. (1990). Musical literacy. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 24, 1-17.
Limbert, W. M. & Polzella, D. J. (1998). Effect of music on the perception of
paintings. Empirical Studies of the Arts. Vol. 16 (1), 33-39.
Lindeman, E.C. (1926). The meaning of adult education. New York: New
Republic.
Lindsay, R. & Vergo, A. (Eds.), (1982). Kandinsky, “Concerning the spiritual in
art.” New York: Springer-Verlag.
Lipscomb, S. D. & Hodges, D. A. ( 1996). Hearing and music perception. In D.
A. Hodges,(ed.) The handbook of music psychology. San Antonio, TX: IMR Press.
Madsen, C. K. (1987). Background music: competition for focus of attention. In
C.K. Madsen & C. A. Prickett (Eds.) Applications for research in music
behavior.(pp.315-325.) Tuscaloosa AL: University of Alabama Press.
Madsen, C. K. & Geringer, J.M. (2000). A focus of attention model for meaningful
listening. Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education, 147, 103-108.
Madsen, C. K. & Geringer, J.M. (1990). Differential patterns of music learning:
focus of attention of musician versus non-musicians. Bulletin for the Council of Research
in Music Education, 105, 45-57.
Madsen, C. K & Wolfe, D. E. (1979). The effect of interrupted music and
incompatible responses on bodily movement and music attentiveness. Journal of Music
Therapy, 16 (1), 36-42.
Masterson, M. L. (1994). Moving beyond “it’s got a good beat.” Music Educators
Journal, 80, (6), 24-28.
Maur, K. (1999). The sound of painting. Munich, New York, NY: Prestel.
Mayberry, M., Parmentier, F.B.R., & Jones, D. M. (2002). Grouping of list items
reflected in the timing of recall: implications for models of serial verbal memory. Journal
of Memory and Language, 47, 360-385.
McAdams, S. (1993). Thinking in sound: The cognitive psychology of human
audiation Oxford: Clarendon Press.
113
McDaniel, M.A. and Pressley, M. (Eds.) (1987). Imagery and related mnemonic
processes. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Meyer, L. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Mitchell, R. W. & Gallagher, M.C. (2001). Embodying music and matching
music and dance in memory. Music Perception, 19. (1), 65-85.
Mullee, M. (1996). Development of a workshop to improve adult music listening
skills. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation: Columbia University.
Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality: principles and implications of cognitive
psychology. New York: Freeman.
Nye, R. E., Nye, V.T., Martin,G.M. & M.L. Rysselberghe. (1992). Music in the
elementary school 6
th
ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Olson, G.B. (1978). Intersensory and intrasensory transfer of melodic contour
perception by children. Journal of Research in Music Education, 26, 41-47.
Olson, G.B. (1981). Perception of melodic contour through intrasensory
matching and intersensory transfer by elementary students. Journal of Education
Research, 74, 358-362.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: a dual-coding approach. NY: Oxford
University Press.
Park, C. C. (2000). Learning style preferences of Southeast Asian students. Urban
Education, 35 (3), 245-268.
Peeck, J. (1987). The role of illustrations in processing and remembering
illustrated text. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton, (Eds.). The psychology of
illustration. volume 1: basic research. New York: Springer- Verlag.
Pomerance, A. (1991). An experimental whole-language in-service workshop for
adult literacy tutors and learners. Philadelphia: The Center for Literacy. (ERIC ED
362780)
Povel, D. & Jansen, E. (2001). Perpetual mechanisms in music possessing. Music
Perception, 19 (2), 169-198.
114
Pressley, M. & Miller G. E., ( 1987). Effects of illustrations on children’s
listening comprehension and oral prose memory. In D.M. Williams & H.A. Houghton
(Eds.) The Psychology of Illustration Vol. 1 Basic Research. New York: Springer-
Verlag.
Purdie, T. (1849) Form and sound: can beauty be dependent on the same
physical laws? Edinburgh.
Radocy, R. & Boyle D., ( 1988). Psychological foundations of musical behavior.
Springfield, IL: C. C . Thomas.
Rakes, G.C. and Rakes, T.A. (1995). Using visuals to enhance secondary
students' reading comprehension of expository texts. Journal of Adolescent and Adult
Literacy, 39, 46-54.
Reimer, B. (1972). The experience of music. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Reimer, B. (1989). Music education and aesthetic education: past and present.
Music Educators Journal, 75, (6), 22-28.
Reimer, B. (1993). Music education in our multimusical culture. Music
Educators Journal, 79 (7), 21-26.
Reimer, B. & Evans, G.(1973). Developing the experience of music: Listening
Charts. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Rentz, E. (1992). Musicians’ and non-musicians’ aural perception of orchestral
instrument families. Journal of Research in Music Education, 40 (3), 185-192.
Ritchey, G.H. (1982). Pictorial detail and recall in adults and children. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 8, 139-141.
Rosner B. S. & Meyer, L. B. (1986). The perceptual roles of melodic process
contour and form. Music Perception, 4 (1), 1-40.
Royal, M.S. & Fiske, H.E. (2000). Cognitive context and musical learning: how
to have it both ways. Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education, 147, 159-
163.
Sanders, D.A. (1977). Auditory perception of speech. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice- Hall.
115
Scheckley, B. G. (1983). Adults learning as a function of temperament ego
maturity and perceived locus of control: How individual differences affect adult learning
projects. Doctoral Dissertation: University of Connecticut.
Scheuller, H. M. (1953). Correspondence between music and the sister arts
according to eighteenth century theory. Journal of Aesthetic and Art Criticism, XI 334-
359.
Schwadron, A. A. (1967). Aesthetics: dimensions of music education.
Washington: MENC.
Shaw-Miller, S. (2002). Visible deeds of music: Art and music from Wagner to
Cage. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Siebenaler, D. J (1999). Student song preference in the elementary music class.
Journal of Research in Music Education. 47, 3, 213-233.
Silver, H., Strong, R, and Perini, M. (1997). Integrating learning styles and
multiple intelligences. Educational Leadership, 55, 22-27
Silver, H.F. and Hanson, J.R. (1995). Learning styles and strategies. Woodbridge,
NJ: The Thoughtful Education Press.
Stolba, K M. ( 1990) The development of western music. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C.
Brown Publishers.
Swanick, K. (1988). Music, mind and education. New York: Routledge.
Tait, M. & Haack, P. (1984). Principles and processes in music education: new
perspectives. New York & London: Teachers College Press.
Thompson, W.F. & Schellenberg, E.G., (2002). Cognitive constraints on music
listening. In Richardson, C. & Colwell, R., (Eds.) The new handbook of research on
music teaching and learning. (p. 461-483) Reston, VA: MENC.
Tough, A. (1979). The adults’ learning project: A fresh approach to theory and
practice in adult learning. 2
nd
Edition. Toronto, Ont: The Ontario Institute for Studies in
Education.
Tulvig, E. (1983,1984). Elements of episodic memory. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Wang, C. & Sogin, D. (1998) Arts infused education in Kentucky. Multimedia
Interactive CD-Rom, The Galef Institute and the National Endowment for the Arts.
116
Wang, C. & Sogin, D. (1998). The role of the arts in different ways of knowing.
Conference Proceedings of the 23
rd
Congress of the International Society for Music
Education, Pretoria, South Africa.
Wang, C. & Sogin, D. (1991). Evaluating the basic arts program: Final report.
Frankfort, KY: The Kentucky Arts Council.
Wenger, R. (1997). Visual art, archeology, and gestalt. Leonardo, 3, 1, 35-46.
Willsdon, C. A. (1996). Klimt’s Beethoven frieze: Goethe, tempelkunst and the
fulfillment of wishes. Art History, 19 (1), 44-73.
Wlodkoski, R. J. (1985). Enhancing adult motivation to learn. San Francisco:
Jossey- Bass.
Wolpert, R.S. (2000). Attention to key in a nondirected music listening task:
musicians and non musicians. Music Perception, 18 (2), 225-230.
Wratcher, M.A. & Jones R.O., (1988). Designing support programs for adult
freshman: A case study using a time management workshop. Paper presented at
conference on freshman year experience. ( ERIC ED 299911).
Yang, C. L. and Wedman, J.F. (1993). A study of the conditions influencing
analogical problem solving. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 26, 213-
221.
117
Vita
Name
Jennifer Sue Shank
Date and Place of Birth
July 20, 1971
Cleveland, Ohio
Degrees Awarded
Master of Music
The University of Kentucky
Lexington, Kentucky
Bachelor of Arts
Shepherd College
Shepherdstown, West Virginia
Orff Certification, Levels I and II
Professional Positions Held
1998-2003 Teaching Assistant
University of Kentucky
Lexington, Kentucky
1998-1999 General Music Education Specialist
St. Marks
Richmond, Kentucky
1995-1998 Band Director
Manassas Park High School
Manassas Park, Virginia
118
1994-1995 General Music Education Specialist
St. Joseph School
Martinsburg, West Virginia
Presentations
The Effect of Musicing on Recognition of Music Concepts by College Students Presented
at the American Orff Schulwerk National Conference, Cincinnati, OH; November 2001
Studying Affective Response in Non-Musicians Presented at the Southern Division of the
Music Educators/Kentucky Music Educators Conference, Louisville KY; Feb 2001
Folk Songs and the World Wide Web Presented at the National Symposium of Music
Instruction Technology at Auburn University, Auburn, AL; July 1999
Membership in Professional Organizations
Music Educators National Conference
Kentucky Music Educators Association
American Orff- Schulwerk Association
Kentucky Orff-Schulwerk Association
College Music Society